Adaptive Behavior and Learning/contents/References

from HTYP, the free directory anyone can edit if they can prove to me that they're not a spambot
Jump to navigation Jump to search
The printable version is no longer supported and may have rendering errors. Please update your browser bookmarks and please use the default browser print function instead.

Title Page
Preface
Chapter 1
Chapter 2
Chapter 3
Chapter 4
Chapter 5
Chapter 6
Chapter 7
Chapter 8
Chapter 9
Chapter 10
Chapter 11
Chapter 12
Chapter 13
Chapter 14
Chapter 15
Chapter 16
References

This article is in need of editing attention:
"Smart" quotes and other special characters did not transcribe properly.

A

  • Allison, J. Paired baseline performance as a behavioral ideal. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1981, 35, 355-366.
  • Allison, J., Miller, M., & Wozny, M. Conservation in behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1979, 108, 4-34.
  • Alloy, L. B., & Seligman, M. E. P. On the cognitive component of learned helplessness and depression. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 13). New York: Academic Press, 1979.
  • Anderson, M. C., & Shettleworth, S. J. Behavioral adaptation to fixed-time food delivery in golden hamsters. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1977, 25, 33-49.
  • Arend, L. E., Buehler, J. N., & Lockhead, G. R. Difference information in brightness perception. Perception and Psychophysics, 1971, 9, 367-370.
  • Ashby, W. R. An introduction to cybernetics. London: Chapman & Hall, 1956.
  • Atkinson, J. W., & Birch, D. The dynamics of action. New York: Wiley, 1970.
  • Attneave, F. Dimensions of similarity. American Journal of Psychology, 1950, 63, 516-556.
  • Autor, S. M. The strength of conditioned reinforcers as a function of frequency and probability of reinforcement. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, 1960.
  • Azrin, N.H., Hutchinson, R. R., & Hake, D. F. Attack, avoidance, and escape reactions to aversive shock. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1967, 10, 131-138.

B

  • Bacotti, A. V. Matching under concurrent fixed-ratio variable-interval schedules of food presentation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1977, 25, 171-182.
  • Barrera, F. J. Centrifugal selection of signal-directed pecking. Journal of the Experimen­tal Analysis of Behavior, 1974, 22, 341-355.
  • Bateson, P. P. G. The imprinting of birds. In S.A. Barnett (Ed.), Ethology and develop­ment. London: Heinemann, 1974.
  • Baum, W. M. Time-based and count-based measurement of preference. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1976, 26, 27-35.
  • Baum, W. M. Understanding behaviorism: science, behavior and culture. New York: HarperCollins, 1994.
  • Baumol, W. J. Economic theory and operations analysis (4th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.
  • Beatty, W. W., & Shavalia, D. A. Rat spatial memory: resistance to retroactive interfer­ence at long retention intervals. Animal Learning and Behavior, 1980, 8, 550-552.
  • Békésy, G. von. Sensory inhibition. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1967.
  • Bell, P. R. (Ed.) Darwin's biological work. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1959.
  • Benson, W. W. Evidence for the evolution of unpalatability through kin selection in the Heliconiinae (Lepidoptera). American Naturalist, 1971, 105, 213-226.
  • Bitterman, M. E. Phyletic differences in learning. American Psychologist, 1965, 20, 396-410.
  • Bitterman, M. E. Habit-reversal and probability learning: rats, birds, and fish. In R. M. Gilbert & N. S. Sutherland (Eds.), Animal discrimination learning. New York: Academic Press, 1969.
  • Donald Blough:
    • Blough, D. S. Steady-state data and a quantitative model of operant generalization and discrimination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1975, 104, 3-21.
    • Blough, D. S. Effects of the number and form of stimuli on visual search in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1979, 5, 211-223.
  • Boakes, R. From Darwin to behaviourism: psychology and the minds of animals. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1984.
  • Bogdany, F. J. Linkage of learning signals in honey bee orientation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 1978, 3, 323-336.
  • Bolles, R. C. Avoidance and escape learning: simultaneous acquisition of different responses. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1969, 68, 355-358.
  • Bolles, R. C. Species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning. Psychological Review, 1970, 77, 32-48.
  • Boren, J. J. Resistance to extinction as a function of the fixed ratio. Journal of Experimen­tal Psychology, 1961, 61, 304-308.
  • Boring, E. G. Sensation and perception in the history of experimental psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1942.
  • Boring, E. G. A history of experimental psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1957.
  • Bouzas, A., & Baum, W. M. Behavioral contrast of time allocation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1976, 25, 179-184.
  • Bower, G. H. & Hilgard, E. R. Theories of learning (5th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1981.
  • Brandt, H. Die lichtorientierung der mehimotte, Ephestia kuehniella. Zeitschrift fur Vergleichende Physiologie, 1934, 20, 646-673.
  • Brazier, M. B. The historical development of neurophysiology. In J. Field, H. Magoun, & V. E. Hall (Eds.), Handbook of physiology Section I. Washington, DC: American Physiological Society, 1959.
  • Breland, K., & Breland, M. The misbehavior of organisms. American Psychologist, 1961, 16, 661-664.
  • Breland, K., & Breland, M. Animal behavior. New York: Macmillan, 1966.
  • Brobeck, J. R. Effects of variations in activity, food intake, and environmental tempera­ture on weight gain in the albino rat. American Journal of Physiology, 1945, 143, 1-5.
  • Brown, P. L., & Jenkins, H. M. Auto-shaping of the pigeon's key-peck. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1968, 11, 1-8.
  • Buck, S. L., Rothstein, B., & Williams, B. A. A re-examination of local contrast in multiple schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1975, 24, 291-301.
  • Buddenbrock, W. von. Mechanismus der phototropen bewegungen. Wissenschafi Meeresuntersuch. N. F. Abteilung Helgoland, 1922, 15, 1-19.
  • Bush, R. R., & Mosteller, F. Stochastic models for learning. New York: Wiley, 1955.
  • Buszâki, G. The "where is it?" reflex: autoshaping the orienting response. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1982, 37, 46l-484.
  • Buszâki, G., Grastyán, B., Winiczai, Z., & Mód, L. Maintenance of signal directed behavior in a response dependent paradigm: a systems approach. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 1979, 39, 201-217.

C

  • Campbell, D. T. On the conflicts between biological and social evolution and between psychology and moral tradition. American Psychologist, 1975, 30, 1103-1126.
  • Caplan, A. I., & Ordahl, C. P. Irreversible gene repression model for the control of development. Science, 1978, 201, 120-130.
  • Caraco, T., Martindale, S., & Whitham, T. S. An empirical demonstration of risk-sensitive foraging preferences. Animal Behaviour, 1980, 28, 820-830.
  • Catania, A. C. Concurrent performances: reinforcement interaction and response inde­pendence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1963, 6, 253-264.
  • Catania, A. C. Self-inhibiting effects of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1973, 19, 517-526.
  • Catania, A. C. Learning. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1979.
  • Catania, A. C. Preference for free choice over forced choice in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1980, 34, 77-86. (a)
  • Catania, A. C. Freedom of choice: a behavioral analysis. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 14). New York: Academic Press, 1980. (b)
  • Catania, A. C. Contingency, contiguity, correlation, and the concept of causation. In P. Harzem & M. D. Zeiler (Eds.), Advances in the study of behaviour. Vol .2: Predictability, correlation and contiguity. Chichester, UK: John Wiley, 1981.
  • Catania, A. C., & Gill, C. A. Inhibition and behavioral contrast. Psychonomic Science, 1964, 1, 257-258.
  • Catania, A. C., & Reynolds, G. S. A quantitative analysis of the behavior maintained by interval schedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1968, 11, 327-383.
  • Catania, A. C., Silverman, P. J., & Stubbs, D. Concurrent performances: stimulus-control gradients during schedules of signalled and unsignalled concurrent reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1974, 21, 99-107.
  • Catania, A.C., Yohalem, R., & Silverman, P.J. Contingency and stimulus change in chained schedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1980, 5, 167-173.
  • Catchpole, C. K. Vocal communication in birds. Baltimore: University Park Press, 1979.
  • Cerella, J. Visual classes and natural categories in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1979, 5, 68-77.
  • Charman, l., & Davison, m. On the effects of food deprivation and component reinforcer rates on multiple-schedule performance. Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 1983, 40, 239-251.
  • Charnov, E. L. Optimal foraging: the marginal value theorem. Theoretical Population Biology, 1976, 9, 129-136.
  • Cherry, C. On human communication. New York: Science Editions, 1961.
  • Chiang, A. C. Fundamental methods of mathematical economics. Tokyo: McGraw-Hill Kogakusha, 1974.
  • Chomsky, N. A review of B. F. Skinner's Verbal behavior. Language, 1959, 35, 26-58.
  • Russell M. Church:
    • Church, R. M. Systematic effect of random error in the yoked control design. Psycholog­ical Bulletin, 1964, 62, 122-131.
    • Church, R. M. The internal clock. In S. H. Hulse, H. Fowler, & W. K. Honig (Eds.), Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1978.
    • Church, R. M. Short-term memory for time intervals. Learning and Motivation, 1980, 11 , 208-219.
  • Collier, G., Hirsch, E., & Hamlin, P. H. The ecological determinants of reinforcement in the rat. Physiology and Behavior, 1972, 9, 705-716.
  • Collier, G., Hirsch, E., & Kanarek, R. The operant revisited. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), The handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.
  • Cook, R. G. (Ed.) Avian visual cognition. Cyberbook: http://www.pigeon.psy.tufts.edu/avc/toc.htm, 2001.
  • Cook, R. M., & Cockrell, B. J. Predator ingestion rate and its bearing on feeding time and the theory of optimal diets. Journal of Animal Ecology, 1978, 47, 529-547.
  • Cooper, L. A. Internal representation. In D. R. Griffin, (Ed.) Animal mind- human mind. Berlin/Heidelberg/New York: Springer-Verlag, 1982.
  • Corning, W., Dyal, J., & Willows, A. O. D. (Eds.) Invertebrate learning (Vols. 1-3). New York: Plenum, 1973.
  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.) The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and generation of culture. New York: Oxford University Press, 1992.
  • Cowie, R. J. Optimal foraging in Great Tits (Parus major). Nature, 1977, 268, 137-139.
  • Crespi, L. P. Quantitative variation in incentive and performance in the white rat. Ameri­can Journal of Psychology, 1942, 5, 467-517.
  • Crovitz, H. F. Galton's Walk: methods for the analysis of thinking, intelligence, and creativity. New York: Harper & Row, 1970.
  • Croze, H. Searching image in carrion crows. Berlin: Paul Parey, 1970.
  • Crozier, W. J., & Cole, W. H. The phototropic excitation of Limax. Journal of General Phvsiology, 1929, 12, 669-674.

D

  • Dale, R. H. I., & Staddon, J. E. R. A temporal theory of spatial memory. Unpublished.
  • D'Amato, M. R. Delayed matching and short-term memory in monkeys. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: advances in research and theory. New York: Academic Press, 1973.
  • Charles R. Darwin:
    • Darwin, C. The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. London: John Murray, 1871.
    • Darwin, C. The origin of species. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1951. (Reprinted from the 6th ed., 1872.)
    • Darwin, C. The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London: John Murray,1872.
    • Darwin, C. The variation of animals and plants under domestication. London: John Murray, 1875. (a)
    • Darwin, C. The movements and habits of climbing plants. London: John Murray, 1875. (b)
    • Darwin, C. The power of movement in plants. London: John Murray, 1880.
  • Davey, M.P., Srinivasan, M.V., & Maddess T. The Craik-O’Brien-Cornsweet illusion in honeybees. Naturwissenschaften, 1998, 85, 73-75.
  • Davies, N. B. Prey selection and the search strategy of the spotted flycatcher (Muscicapa striata), a field study on optimal foraging. Animal Behaviour, 1977, 25, 1016-1033.
  • Davies, N. B., & Houston, A. I. Owners and satellites: the economics of territory defence in the Pied Wagtail, Motacilla alba. Journal of Animal Ecology, 1981, 50, 157-180.
  • Davis, J. D., & Levine, M. W. A model for the control of ingestion. Psychological Review, 1977, 84, 379-4l2.
  • Davis, M. Socially induced flight reactions in pigeons. Animal Behaviour, 1975, 23, 597-601.
  • Dawkins, M. Perceptual changes in chicks, another look at the “search image" concept. Animal Behaviour, 1971, 19, 566-574. (a)
  • Dawkins, M. Shifts of "attention" in chicks during feeding. Animal Behaviour, 1971, 19, 575-582. (b)
  • Dennett, D. C. Brainstorms. Montgomery, VT: Bradford, 1978.
  • de Ruiter, L. Some experiments on the camouflage of stick caterpillars. Behaviour, 1952, 4, 222-232.
  • de Villiers, P. A. The law of effect and avoidance: a quantitative relationship between response rate and shock-frequency reduction. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1974, 21, 223-235.
  • de Villiers, P. A. Choice in concurrent schedules and a quantitative formulation of the Law of Effect. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.
  • de Villiers, P. A., & Herrnstein, R. J. Toward a law of response strength. Psychological Bulletin, 1976, 83, 1131-1153.
  • Dickinson, A. Contemporary animal learning theory. Cambridge, UK/New York: Cambridge University Press, 1980.
  • Dickinson, A., Hall, G., & Mackintosh, N. J. Surprise and the attenuation of blocking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1976, 2, 213-222.
  • Dickinson, A., & Mackintosh, N.J. Classical conditioning in animals. Annual Review of Psychology, 1978, 29, 587-612.
  • Domjan, M. Ingestional aversion learning: unique and general processes. In J. S. Rosenblatt, R. A. Hinde, & M. C. Busnel (Eds.), Advances in the study of behavior (Vol. 11). New York: Academic Press, 1980.
  • Dunham, P. J. Punishment: method and theory. Psychological Review, 1971, 78, 58-70.
  • Dunham, P. J. The nature of reinforcing stimuli. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.

E

  • Edelman, G. Neural Darwinism: the theory of neuronal group selection. New York: Basic Books, 1987.
  • Edmon, E. L. Multiple schedule component duration: a reanalysis of Shimp and Wheatley (1971) and Todorov (1972). Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1978, 30, 239-241.
  • Einhorn, H. J., & Hogarth, R. M. Confidence in judgment: persistence of the illusion of validity. Psychological Review, 1978, 85, 395-416.
  • Ekman, G. Dimensions of color vision. Journal of Psychology, 1954, 38, 467-474.
  • Elliffe, d., & Davison, m. Performance in continuously available multiple schedules. Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 1985, 44, 343-353.
  • Ellison, G. D., & Konorski, J. Separation of the salivary and motor responses in instru­mental conditioning. Science, 1964, 146, 1071-1072.
  • Estes, W. K. Effects of competing reactions on the conditioning curve for bar-pressing. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1950, 40, 200-205.
  • Estes, W. K. The problem of inference from curves based on group data. Psychological Bulletin, 1956, 53, 134-140.
  • Estes, W. K. The statistical approach to learning theory. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: a study of a science (Vol.2). New York: McGraw-Hill, 1959.
  • Estes, W. K., & Skinner, B. F. Some quantitative properties of anxiety. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1941, 29, 390-400.
  • Estes, W. K., Koch, S., MacCorquodale, K., Meehl, P. E., Mueller, C. G., Schoenfeld, W. N., & Verplanck, W. S. Modern learning theory. New York: Appleton-Century­-Crofts, 1954.
  • Ettinger, R. H., & Staddon, J. E. R. The operant regulation of feeding: a static analysis. Behavioral Neuroscience, 1983, 97, 639-653.

F

  • Falk, J. L. Production of polydipsia in normal rats by an intermittent food schedule. Science, 1961, 133, 195-196.
  • Fantino, E. Choice and rate of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1969,12, 723-730.
  • Fantino, E. Conditioned reinforcement. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs. NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.
  • Fantino, E. Contiguity, response strength, and the delay-reduction hypothesis. In P. Harzem & M.D. Zeiler (Eds.), Advances in the study of behaviour. Vol. 2: Predictability, correlation, and contiguity. Chichester, UK: Wiley, 1981.
  • Fantino, E., & Logan, C. A. The experimental analysis of behavior: a biological per­spective. San Francisco: Freeman, 1979.
  • Farley, J. Automaintenance, contrast, and contingencies: effects of local vs. overall and prior vs. impending reinforcement context. Learning and Motivation, 1980, 11, l9-48.
  • Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. Schedules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton-­Century-Crofts, 1957.
  • Fletcher, H. J. The delayed-response problem. In A. M. Schrier, H. F. Harlow, & F. Stollnitz (Eds.), Behavior of nonhuman primates. New York: Academic Press, 1965.
  • Ford, E. B. Polymorphism. Biological Review, 1945, 20, 73-88.
  • Fraenkel, G. S. Die mechanik der orientierung der tiere im raum. Biological Review, 1931, 6, 36-87.
  • Fraenkel, G. S., & Gunn, D. L. The orientation of animals. Fair Lawn, NJ: Oxford University Press, 1940. (Dover edition: 1961.)
  • Frank, J., & Staddon, J. E. R. The effects of restraint on temporal discrimination behav­ior. Psychological Record, 1974, 23, 123-130.
  • Friedman, M., & Savage, J. The utility analysis of choices involving risk. Journal of Political Economy, 1948, 56, 279-304.
  • Friedman, M. I., & Stricker, E.M. The physiological psychology of hunger: a physiolog­ical perspective. Psychological Review, 1976, 83, 409-431.

G

  • Gallistel, C. R. The organization of action: a new synthesis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1990.
  • Gallistel, C. R., & Gibbon, J. The symbolic foundations of conditioned behavior. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Associates, 2002.
  • Garcia, J. Tilting at the paper mills of academe. American Psychologist, 1981, 36, 149-158.
  • Garcia, J., Clarke, J., & Hankins, W. G. Natural responses to scheduled rewards. In P. P. G. Bateson & P. Klopfer (Eds.), Perspectives in ethology. New York: Plenum, 1973.
  • Garcia, J., Kimeldorf, D. J., & Koelling, R. A. A conditioned aversion towards saccharin resulting from exposure to gamma radiation. Science, 1955, 122, 157-159.
  • Gendron, R. P. Foraging for cryptic prey. Doctoral dissertation, Duke University, 1982.
  • Gendron, R. P., & Staddon, J. E. R. Searching for cryptic prey: the effect of search speed. American Naturalist, 1983, 121, 172-186.
  • Gibbon, J. Scalar expectancy and Weber's law in animal timing. Psychological Review, 1977, 84, 279-325.
  • Gibbon, J., Berryman, R., & Thompson, R. L. Contingency spaces and measures in classical conditioning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1974, 21, 585-605.
  • Gibbon, J., Locurto, C., & Terrace, H. Signal food contingency and signal food fre­quency in a continuous-trials autoshaping paradigm. Animal Learning and Behavior, 1975, 3, 313-324.
  • Gibbs, C. M., Latham, S. B., & Gormezano, I. Classical conditioning of the rabbit nictitating membrane response: effects of reinforcement schedule on response maintenance and resistance to extinction. Animal Learning and Behavior, 1978, 6, 209-215.
  • Gigerenzer, G., & Selten, R. (Eds.) Bounded rationality: The adaptive toolbox. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2002.
  • Gill, F. B., & Wolf, L. L. Economics of feeding territoriality in the golden-winged sunbird. Ecology, 1978, 56, 333-345.
  • Glaister, S. Mathematical methods for economists. London: Gray-Mills, 1972.
  • Glazer, H. I., & Weiss, J. M. Long-term interference effect: an alternative to "learned helplessness." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1976, 2, 201-213.
  • Gottlieb, G. Development of species identification in birds. Chicago: University of Chi­cago Press, 1971.
  • Gottlieb, G. On the acoustic basis of species identification in wood ducklings (Aix sponsa). Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1974, 87, 1038-1048.
  • Gould, J. L., & Gould, C. G. The animal mind. New York: W. H. Freeman, 1999.
  • Gould, S. J. Ontogeny and phylogeny. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1977.
  • grace, r. c. a contextual model of concurrent-chains choice. journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 1994, 61, 113-129.
  • Granit, R. Charles Scott Sherrington: an appraisal. London: Nelson, 1966.
  • Grant, D. S. Delayed alternation in the rat: effect of contextual stimuli on proactive interference. Learning and Motivation, 1980, 11, 339-354.
  • Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley, 1966.
  • Green, L., & Snyderman, M. Choice between rewards differing in amount and delay: toward a choice model of self control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1980, 34, 135-147.
  • Greenwood, M. R. C., Quartermain, D., Johnson, R. R., Cruce, J. A. F., & Hirsch, J. Food motivated behavior in genetically obese and hypothalamic-hyperphagic rats and mice. Physiology and Behavior, 1974, 13, 687-692.
  • Grindley, G. C. The formation of a simple habit in guinea pigs. British Journal of Psychology, 1932, 23, 127-147.
  • Grodins, F. S. Control theory and biological systems. New York: Columbia University Press, 1963.
  • Grossberg, S. Psychophysiological substrates of schedule interactions and behavioral contrast. SIAM-AMS Proceedings, 1981, 13, 157-186.
  • Grossberg, S. Studies of mind and brain. Boston: D. Reidel, 1982.
  • Guttman, N. Effects of discrimination formation on generalization. In D. I. Mostofsky (Ed.), Stimulus generalization. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press, 1965.
  • Guttman, N., & Kalish, H. I. Discriminability and stimulus generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1956, 51, 79-88.

H

  • Hall, G., & Pearce, J. M. Latent inhibition of a CS during CS-US pairings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1979, 5, 3l-42.
  • Hamilton, B. E., & Silberberg, A. Contrast and autoshaping in multiple schedules vary­ing reinforcer rate and duration. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1978, 30, 107-122.
  • Hanson, H. M. The effects of discrimination training on stimulus generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1959, 58, 321-334.
  • Harlow, H. F. The formation of learning sets. Psychological Review, 1949, 56, 51-65.
  • Harlow, H. F., & Harlow, M. K. The affectional systems. In A. M. Schrier, H. F. Harlow, & F. Stollnitz (Eds.), Behavior of nonhuman primates (Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press, 1965.
  • Hassell, M. P. The dynamics of competition and predation. London: Edward Arnold, 1976.
  • Hassell, M. P. The dynamics of arthropod predator-prey systems. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press, 1978.
  • Hatten, J. L., & Shull, R. L. Pausing on fixed-interval schedules: effects of the prior feeder duration. Behaviour Analysis Letters, 1983, 3, 101-111.
  • Hearst, E. The classical-instrumental distinction: reflexes, voluntary behavior, and cate­gories of associative learning. In W. K. Estes (Ed.), Handbook of learning and cognitive processes (Vol.2). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1975.
  • Hearst, E. Stimulus relationships and feature selection in learning and behavior. In S. H. Hulse, H. Fowler, & W. K. Honig (Eds.), Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1978.
  • Hearst, E. The first century of experimental psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1979.
  • Hearst, E., & Jenkins, H. M. Sign-tracking: the stimulus-reinforcer relation and directed action. Psychonomic Society Monograph, 1974.
  • Hearst, E., Koresko, M. B., & Poppen, R. Stimulus generalization and the response-reinforcement contingency. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1964, 7, 369-380.
  • Heinroth, O. Beiträge zur Biologie, namentlich Ethologie und Psychologie der Anatiden. Proceedings of the 5th International Ornithological Congress, 1911, 589-702.
  • Henderson, J. M., & Quandt, R. E. Microeconomic theory: a mathematical approach. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1971.
  • Henton, W. W., & Iversen, I. H. Classical conditioning and operant conditioning. New York: Springer-Verlag, 1978.
  • Richard J. Herrnstein:
    • Herrnstein, R. J. Relative and absolute strength of response as a function of frequency of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1961, 4, 267-272.
    • Herrnstein, R. J. Secondary reinforcement and rate of primary reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1964, 7, 27-36.
    • Herrnstein, R. J. On the law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1970, 13, 243-266.
    • Herrnstein, R. J. Self control as response strength. In C. M. Bradshaw, C. F. Lowe, & E. Szabadi (Eds.), Recent Developments in the quantification of steady-state operant behavior. Amsterdam: Elsevier/North-Holland, 1981.
    • Herrnstein, R. J. The matching law. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1997.
    • Herrnstein, R. J., & Boring, E. G. A source book in the history of psychology. Cam­bridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1965.
    • Herrnstein, R. J., & Heyman, G. M. Is matching compatible with reinforcement maximi­zation on concurrent variable interval, variable ratio? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1979, 31, 209-223.
    • Herrnstein, R. J., & Loveland, D. H. Complex visual concept in the pigeon. Science, 1964, 146, 549-551.
    • Herrnstein, R. J., & Loveland, D. H. Hunger and contrast in a multiple schedule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1974, 21, 511-517.
    • Herrnstein, R. J., & Loveland, D. H. Maximizing and matching on concurrent ratio schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1975, 24, 107-116.
    • Herrnstein, R. J., Loveland, D. H., & Cable, C. Natural concepts in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1976, 2, 285-302.
  • Hess, E. H. Space perception in the chick. Scientific American, 1956, 195, 71-80.
  • Hess, E. H. Imprinting. New York: Van Nostrand/Reinhold, 1973.
  • Hetherington, A. W., & Ranson, S. W. Hypothalamic lesions and adiposity in the rat. Anatomical Record, 1940, 78, 149-172.
  • Hetherington, A. W., & Ranson, S. W. The spontaneous activity and food intake of rats with hypothalamic lesions. American Journal of Physiology, 1942, 136, 609-617.
  • Heyman, G. M. Matching and maximizing in concurrent schedules. Psychological Re­view, 1979, 86, 496-500.
  • Hicks, J. R. A revision of demand theory. Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1956.
  • Hilgard, E. R., & Marquis, D. G. Conditioning and learning. New York: Appleton-­Century-Crofts, 1940.
  • Hinde, R. A. Bird vocalizations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1969.
  • Hinde, R. A. Animal behaviour: a synthesis of ethology and comparative psychology (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill, 1970.
  • Hinde, R. A., & Hinde, J. (Eds.) Constraints on learning: limitations and predisposi­tions. New York: Academic Press, 1973.
  • Hineline, P. N. Negative reinforcement and avoidance. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.
  • Hinson, J. M., & Staddon, J. E. R. Behavioral competition: a mechanism for schedule interactions. Science, 1978, 202, 432-434.
  • Hinson, J. M., & Staddon, J. E. R. Maximizing on interval schedules. In C. M. Bradshaw, C. F. Lowe, & E. Szabadi (Eds.), Recent developments in the quantification of steady-state operant behavior. Amsterdam: Elsevier/North-Holland, 1981.
  • Hinson, J. M., & Staddon, J. E. R. Hill-climbing by pigeons. Journal of the Experimen­tal Analysis of Behavior, 1983, 39, 25-47.
  • Hirsch, E., & Collier, G. The ecological determinants of reinforcement in the guinea pig. Physiology and Behavior, 1974, 12, 239-249.
  • Hodos, W., & Trumbule, G. H. Strategies of schedule preference in chimpanzees. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1967, 10, 503-514.
  • Hoebel, B. G., & Teitelbaum, P. Weight regulation in normal and hypothalamic hyper­phagic rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1966, 61, 189-193.
  • Hogan, J. A. How young chicks learn to recognize food. In R. A. Hinde & J. Stevenson­-Hinde (Eds.), Constraints on learning. New York: Academic Press, 1973.
  • Hogan, J. A., Kleist, S., & Hutchings, C. S. L. Display and food as reinforcers in the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens). Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1970, 70, 351-357.
  • Hogan, J. A., & Roper, T. J. A comparison of the properties of different reinforcers. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 1978, 8, 156-255.
  • Hollings, C. S. The components of predation, as revealed by a study of small mammal predation of the European pine sawfly. Canadian Entomologist, 1959, 91, 293-332.
  • Hollings, C. S. The functional response of predators to prey density and its role in mimicry and population regulation. Memoirs of the Entomological Society of Canada, 1965, 45, 5-60.
  • Holman, G. L. Intragastric reinforcement effect. Journal of Comparative and Physiologi­cal Psychology, 1969, 69, 432-441.
  • Honig, W. K. Studies of working memory in the pigeon. In S. H. Hulse, H. Fowler, & W. K. Honig (Eds.), Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1978.
  • Honig, W. K., Boneau, C. A., Burstein, K. R., & Pennypacker, H. S. Positive and negative generalization gradients obtained after equivalent training conditions. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1963, 56, 111-116.
  • Honig, W. K., & Urcuioli, P. J. The legacy of Guttman and Kalish (1956): 25 years of research on stimulus generalization. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1981, 36, 405-455.
  • Houston, A. I., & McNamara, J. How to maximize reward rate on two variable-interval paradigms. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1981, 35, 367-396.
  • Hovland, C. I. Human learning and retention. In S. S. Stevens (Ed.), Handbook of experimental psychology. New York: Wiley, 1951.
  • Hull, C. L. Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1943.
  • Hull, C. L. A behavior system. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1952.
  • Hull, C. L., Hovland, C. I., Ross, R. T., Hall, M., Perkins, D. T., & Fitch, F. B. Mathematico-deductive theory of rote learning. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1940.
  • Hulse, S. H., Deese, J., & Egeth, H. The psychology of learning. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1975.
  • Huxley, J. Problems of relative growth. London: Methuen, 1932. (Dover edition, 1972.).

I

  • Immelman, K. Sexual imprinting in birds. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 1972, 4,147-174.
  • Ingle, D. Focal attention in the frog: behavioral and physiological correlates. Science ,1975, 188, 1033-1035.
  • Intriligator, M. D. Mathematical optimization and economic theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1971.

J

  • Jenkins, H. M. Generalization gradients and the concept of inhibition. In D. I. Mostofsky (Ed.), Stimulus generalization. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press, 1965.
  • Jenkins, H. M., & Sainsbury, R. S. Discrimination learning with the distinctive feature on positive or negative trials. In D. I. Mostofsky (Ed.), Attention: contemporary theory and analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1970.
  • Jennings, H. S. Behavior of the lower organisms. Bloomington: Indiana University Press, 1976. (Reprint of the 1906 edition.)

K

  • Kacelnik, A., & Bateson, M. Risk-sensitivity: cross-roads for theories of decision-making. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1997, 1, 304-309.
  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. Prospect theory: an analysis of decision-making under risk. Econometrica, 1979, 47, 263-291.
  • Kamil, A. C. Systematic foraging by a nectar-feeding bird, the Amakihi (Loxops virens). Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1978, 92, 388-396.
  • Kamil, A. C., & Sargent, T. D. (Eds.) Foraging behavior: ecological, ethological, and psychological approaches. New York: Garland Press, 1981.
  • Kamin, L. J. Selective attention and conditioning. In N. J. Mackintosh & W. K. Honig (Eds.), Fundamental issues in associative learning. Halifax: Dalhousie University Press, 1969.
  • Keesey, R. E., & Powley, T. L. Hypothalamic regulation of body weight. American Scientist, 1975, 63, 558-565.
  • Keith-Lucas, T., & Guttman, N. Robust-single-trial delayed backward conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1975, 88, 468-476.
  • Kelleher, R. T. Chaining and conditioned reinforcement. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: areas of research and application. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1966.
  • Kello, J. E. The reinforcement-omission effect on fixed-interval schedules: frustration or inhibition? Learning Motivation, 1972, 3, 138-147.
  • Kello, J. Observation of the behavior of rats running to reward and nonreward in an alleyway. Doctoral dissertation, Duke University, 1973.
  • Kello, J. E., Innis, N. K., & Staddon, J. E. R. Eccentric stimuli on multiple fixed-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1975, 23, 233-240.
  • Kelsey, J. E., & Allison, J. Fixed ratio lever pressing by VMH rats: work vs. accessibility of sucrose reward. Physiology and Behavior, 1976, 17, 749-754.
  • Kennedy, G. C. The hypothalamic control of food intake in rats. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 1950, 137, 535-549.
  • Kennedy, J. S. Coordination of successive activities in an aphid: reciprocal effects of settling on flight. Journal of Experimental Biology, 1965, 43, 489-509.
  • Kennedy, J. S. Behaviour as physiology. In L. B. Browne (Ed.), Insects and physiology. New York: Springer-Verlag, 1967.
  • Killeen, P. R. On the temporal control of behavior. Psychological Review, 1975, 82, 89-115.
  • Killeen, P. R. Superstition: a matter of bias, not detectability. Science, 1978, 199, 88-90.
  • Killeen, P. R. Incentive theory. In D. Bernstein (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motiva­tion, 1981. Lincoln: Nebraska University Press, 1982.
  • Killeen, P. R. Incentive theory: II. models for choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1982, 38, 217-232.
  • Killeen, P. Complex dynamic processes in sign tracking with an omission contingency (negative automaintenance). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2003, 29, 49-61.
  • Killeen, P. R., & Fantino, E. Unification of models for choice between delayed reinforcers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1990, 53, 189-200.
  • Killeen, P. R., Hanson, S. J., & Osborne, S. R. Arousal: its genesis and manifestation as response rate. Psychological Review, 1978, 85, 571-581.
  • Kimble, G. A. Hilgard and Marquis' conditioning and learning (2nd ed). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1961.
  • Kimble, G. A., & Perlmutter, L. C. The problem of volition. Psychological Review, 1970, 77, 361-384.
  • Klopfer, P. H., & Gamble, J. Maternal "imprinting" in goats: the role of chemical senses. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 1966, 25, 588-592.
  • Koch, S. (Ed.) Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. 2). New York: McGraw-Hill, 1959.
  • Kolata, G. B. Obesity: a growing problem. Science, 1977, 198, 905-906.
  • Konishi, M., & Hauser, M. (Eds.) The design of animal communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1999.
  • Konishi, M., & Nottebohm, F. Experimental studies in the ontogeny of avian vocaliza­tions. In R. A. Hinde (Ed.), Bird vocalizations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1969.
  • Koshland, D. B. A response regulator model in a simple sensory system. Science, 1977, 196, 1055-1063.
  • Koshland, D. E. Bacterial chemotaxis as a model behavioral system. New York: Raven Press, 1980.
  • Kraly, F. S., & Blass, E. M. Increased feeding in rats in a low ambient temperature. In D. Novin, W. Wyrwicka, & G. A. Bray (Eds.), Hunger: basic mechanisms and clinical implications. New York: Raven Press, 1976.
  • Krebs, J. R. Behavioural aspects of predation. In P. P. G. Bateson & P. H. Klopfer (Eds.), Perspectives in ethology. New York: Plenum, 1973.
  • Krebs, J. R. Optimal foraging: decision rules for predators. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer, 1978.
  • Krebs, J. R., & Davies, N. B. (Eds.) Behavioural ecology. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer, 1978.
  • Krebs, J. R., & Davies, N. B. An introduction to behavioural ecology. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer, 1981.
  • Krebs, J. R., Kacelnik, A., & Taylor, P. Tests of optimal sampling by foraging great tits. Nature, 1978, 275, 27-31.
  • Kreider, D. L., Kuller, R. G., Ostberg, D. R., & Perkins, F. W. An introduction to linear analysis. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley, 1966.
  • Kroodsma, D. A reevaluation of song development in the song sparrow. Animal Behaviour, 1977, 25, 390-399.
  • Kroodsma, D. A. Aspects of learning in the ontogeny of bird song: where, from whom, when, how much, which, and how accurately? In G. Burghardt & M. Bekoff (Eds.), Development of behavior. New York: Garland Press, 1978.
  • Kuo, Z-Y. The dynamics of behavior development: an epigenetic view. New York: Random House, 1967.
  • Kuo, Z-Y. The need for coordinated efforts in developmental studies. In L. R. Aronson, E. Tobach, D. S. Lehrrnan, & J. S. Rosenblatt (Eds.), Development and evolution of behavior: essays in memory of T. C. Schneirla. San Francisco: Freeman, 1970.

L

  • Lancaster, K. Mathematical economics. New York: Macmillan, 1968.
  • Land, E. H., & McCann, J. J. Lightness and retinex theory. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 1971, 61, 1-11.
  • Landauer, T. K. Reinforcement as consolidation. Psychological Review, 1969, 76, 82-96.
  • Lashley, K. S. The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms and behavior. New York: Wiley, 1951.
  • Lea, S. E. G. Titration of schedule parameters by pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1976, 25, 43-54.
  • Lea, S. E. G. Concurrent fixed-ratio schedules for different reinforcers: a general theory. In C. M. Bradshaw, E. Szabadi, & C. F. Lowe (Eds.), Quantification of steady-state operant behavior. Amsterdam: Elsevier/North-Holland, 1981.
  • Lea, S. E. G., & Roper, T. J. Demand for food on fixed-ratio schedules as a function of the quality of concurrently available reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1977, 27, 371-380.
  • Lehrman, D. S. Semantic and conceptual issues in the nature-nurture problem. In L. R. Aronson, E. Tobach, D. S. Lehrman, & J. S. Rosenblatt (Eds.), Development and evolution of behavior: essays in memory of T. C. Schneirla. San Francisco: Freeman, 1970.
  • Lett, B. T. Long-delay learning in the T-maze. Learning and Motivation, 1975, 6, 80-90.
  • Lewin, R. Seeds of change in embryonic development. Science, 1981, 214, 42-44.
  • Lockhead, G. R. Identification and the form of multidimensional discrimination space. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1970, 85, 1-10.
  • Lockhead, G. R. Processing dimensional stimuli: a note. Psychological Review, 1972, 79, 410-419.
  • Locurto, C., Terrace, H. S., & Gibbon, J. (Eds.) Autoshaping and conditioning theory. New York: Academic Press, 1981.
  • Loeb, J. Forced movements, tropisms, and animal conduct. Philadelphia: Lippincott, 1918. (Dover edition, 1973.)
  • Lolordo, V. M., McMillan, J. C., & Riley, A. L. The effects upon food-reinforced pecking and treadle-pressing of auditory and visual signals for response-independent food. Learning and Motivation, 1974, 5, 24-41.
  • Lorenz, K. Companions as factors in the bird's environment. In R. Martin (trans.), Studies in animal and human behaviour, Vol. I. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1970.
  • Lowe, C. F., Davey, G. C., & Harzem, P. Effects of reinforcement magnitude on interval and ratio schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1974, 22, 553-560.
  • Lucas, C. A. The control of keypecks during automaintenance by pre-keypeck omission training. Animal Learning and Behavior, 1975, 3, 33-36.
  • Luce, R. D. Individual choice behavior: a theoretical analysis. New York: Wiley, 1959.
  • Luce, R. D. Detection and recognition. In R. D. Luce, R. R. Bush, & E. Galanter (Eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Wiley, 1963.
  • Luce, R. D. The choice axiom after twenty years. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 1977, 15, 215-233.
  • Luce, R. D., Bush, R. R., & Galanter, E. Handbook of mathematical psychology (3 vols.). New York: Wiley, 1963.
  • Luce, R. D., & Raiffa, H. Games and decisions. New York: Wiley, 1957.
  • Ludlow, A. R. The behaviour of a model animal. Behaviour, 1976, 108, 131-172.
  • Ludlow, A. R. The evolution and simulation of a decision maker. In F. M. Toates & T. R. Halliday (Eds.), The analysis of motivational processes. London: Academic Press, 1980.

M

  • MacColl, L. A. Fundamental theory of servomechanisms. New York: Van Nostrand, 1945.
  • MacEwen, D. The effects of terminal-link fixed-interval and variable-interval schedules on responding under concurrent chained schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1972, 18, 253-261.
  • Mackintosh, N. J. Comparative psychology of serial reversal and probability learning: rats, birds, and fish. In R. Gilbert & N. S. Sutherland (Eds.), Animal discrimination learning. London: Academic Press, 1969.
  • Mackintosh, N.J. The psychology of animal learning. New York: Academic Press, 1974. Mackintosh, N. J. A theory of animal attention: variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Review, 1975, 82, 276-298.
  • Mackintosh, N. J. Stimulus control: attentional factors. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.
  • Mackintosh, N. J., Bygrave, D. J., & Picton, B. M. B. Locus of the effects of a surprising reinforcer in the attenuation of blocking. Quarterly Journal of Psychol­ogy, 1977, 29, 327-336.
  • Macnab, R. M., & Koshland, D. E. The gradient-sensing mechanism in bacterial chemo­taxis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 1972, 69, 2509-2512.
  • Magnus, D. Experimentelle untersuchungen zur bionomie und ethologie des kaisermantels Argynnispaphia L. (Lep. Nymph.) Zeitschrift fürTierpsychologie, 1958, 15, 397-426.
  • Mahoney, W. J., & Ayres, J. J. B. One-trial simultaneous and backward fear condition­ing as reflected in conditioned suppression of licking in rats. Animal Learning and Behavior, 1976, 4, 357-362.
  • Maier, S. F., & Jackson, R. L. Learned helplessness: all of us were right (and wrong): learned helplessness has multiple effects. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 13). New York: Academic Press, 1979.
  • Malone, J. C., & Staddon, J. E. R. Contrast effects in maintained generalization gradi­ents. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1973, 19, 167-179.
  • Marler, P. R., & Hamilton, W. J. Mechanisms of animal behavior. New York: Wiley, 1966.
  • Marler, P., Kreith, M., & Tamura, M. Song development in hand-raised Oregon juncos. Auk, 1962, 79, 12-30.
  • Marler, P., & Tamura, M. Culturally transmitted patterns of vocal behavior in sparrows. Science, 1964, 46, 1483-1486.
  • Marshall, A. Principles of economics (8th ed.). London: Macmillan, 1925.
  • Marwine, A., & Collier, G. The rat at the waterhole. Journal of Comparative and Physio­logical Psychology, 1979, 93, 39l-402.
  • Mast, S. O. Light and the behavior of organisms. New York: Wiley, 1911.
  • Maxwell, J. C. On governors. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, 1868, 16, 270-283.
  • May, R. M. Simple mathematical models with very complicated dynamics. Nature, 1976, 261, 459-467.
  • Maynard Smith, J. Evolution and the theory of games. Scientific American, 1976, 64, 41-45.
  • Maynard Smith, J. Theory in evolution. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 1978, 9, 31-56.
  • McCleery, R. On satiation curves. Animal Behaviour, 1977, 25, 1005-1015.
  • McCleery, R. Optimal behaviour sequences and decision making. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural ecology. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer, 1978.
  • McFarland, D. J. Feedback mechanisms in animal behaviour. New York: Academic Press, 1971.
  • McFarland, D. J. (Ed.) Motivational control systems analysis. London: Academic Press, 1974.
  • McFarland, D. J., & Houston, A. Quantitative ethology: the state space approach. London: Pitman, 1981.
  • McGinty, D., Epstein, A. N., & Teitelbaum, P. The contribution of oropharyngeal sensations to hypothalamic hyperphagia. Animal Behaviour, 1965, 13, 413-418.
  • McKearney, J. W. Fixed-interval schedules of electric shock presentation: extinction and recovery of performance under different shock intensities and fixed-interval dura­tions. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1969, 12, 301-313.
  • McLean, A. P., & White, K. G. Temporal constraint on choice: Sensitivity and bias in multiple schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1983, 39, 405-426.
  • McNamara, J., & Houston, A. I. The application of statistical decision theory to animal behaviour. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 1980, 85, 673-690.
  • McNamera, J.M., & Houston, A. I. Models of adaptive behavior: an approach based on state. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1999.
  • McSweeney, F. K., & Melville, C. L. Behavioral contrast for key pecking as a function of component duration when only one component varies. Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 1993, 60, 331-343.
  • Medin, D. L. Form perception and pattern reproduction by monkeys. Journal of Compar­ative and Physiological Psychology, 1969, 68, 412-419.
  • Meschowski, H. Ways of thought of great mathematicians. San Francisco: Holden-Day, 1964.
  • Miles, R. C. Discrimination-learning sets. In A. M. Schrier, H. F. Harlow, & F. Stollnitz (Eds.), Behavior of nonhuman primates (Vol. I). New York: Academic Press, 1965.
  • Milich, R. S. A critical analysis of Schachter's externality theory of obesity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1975, 84, 586-588.
  • Miller, G.A. The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 1956, 63, 81-97.
  • Miller, N. E., & Banuazizi, A. Instrumental learning by curarized rats of a specific vis­ceral response. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1968, 65, 1-7.
  • Miller, N. E., & Dworkin, B. R. Visceral learning: recent difficulties with curarized rats and significant problems for human research. In P. A. Obrist, A. H. Black, J. Brener, & L. V. DiCara (Eds.), Cardiovascular physiology: current issues in re­sponse mechanisms, biofeedback, and methodology. Chicago: Aldine, 1974.
  • Milsum, J. H. Biological control systems analysis. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1966.
  • Minsky, M. Computation: finite and infinite machines. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1967.
  • MIT Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science. http://cognet.mit.edu/MITECS/Front/introduction.html.
  • Mittelstaedt, H. Basic solutions to a problem of angular orientation. In R. F. Reiss (Ed.), Neural theory and modeling. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press, 1964.
  • Mittelstaedt, H. Kybernetik der schwereorientierung. Verhandlungen der Deutschen Zoo­logischen Gesellschaft, 1972, 185-200.
  • Moore, E. F. Gedanken-experiments on sequential machines. In C. E. Shannon & J. McCarthy (Eds.), Automata studies. Princeton, NJ, Princeton Annals of Mathe­matics Studies, 1956.
  • Moore, G. E. Principia ethica. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1903.
  • Moorhouse, J. E., Fosbrooke, I. H. M., & Kennedy, J. S. "Paradoxical driving" of walking activity in locusts. Journal of Experimental Biology, 1978, 72, 1-16.
  • Morse, W., & Kelleher, R. Determinants of reinforcement and punishment. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.
  • Mrosovsky, N., & Powley, T. L. Set points for body weight and fat. Behavioral Biology, 1977, 20, 205-223.
  • Müller, A. Ãœber lichtreaktionen von landasseln. Zeitschrift für Vergleichende Physiologie, 1925, 3, 113-144.
  • Mulligan, J. A. Singing behavior and its development in the song sparrow, Melospiza melodia. University of California Publications in Zoology, 1966, 81, 1-76.
  • Murdoch, W. W. Switching in general predators: experiments on predator specificity and stability of prey populations. Ecological Monographs, 1969, 39, 335-354.
  • Murdoch, W. W., & Oaten, A. Predation and population stability. In A. Macfadyen (Ed.), Advances in ecological research (Vol. 9). New York: Academic Press, 1975.
  • Myerson, J., & Miezin, F. M. The kinetics of choice: an operant systems analysis. Psychological Review, 1980, 87, 160-174.

N

  • Navarick, D. J., & Fantino, E. Self-control and general models of choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1976, 2, 75-87.
  • Neimark, E. D., & Estes, W. K. Stimulus sampling theory. San Francisco: Holden-Day, 1967.
  • Neuringer, A. J., & Chung, S.-H. Quasi-reinforcement: control of responding by a percentage reinforcement schedule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behav­ior, 1967, 10, 45-54.
  • Nevin, J. A. Response strength in multiple schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analy­sis of Behavior, 1974, 21, 389-408.
  • Nevin, J. A. Overall matching versus momentary maximizing: Nevin (1969) revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1979a, 5, 300-306.
  • Nevin, J. A. Reinforcement schedules and response strength. In M. D. Zeiler, and P. Harzem (Eds.), Reinforcement and the organization of behavior. New York: Wiley, 1979b.
  • Nevin, J. A., & Grace, R. C. Behavioral momentum and the law of effect. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2000, 23, 73-130.
  • Nevin, J. A., & Shettleworth, S. J. An analysis of contrast effects in multiple schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1966, 9, 305-315.
  • Nevin, J. A., Tota, M. E., Torquato, R. D., & Shull, R. L. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1990, 53, 359-379.
  • Nicolaïdis, S., & Rowland, N. Intravenous self-feeding: long-term regulation of energy balance in rats. Science, 1977, 195, 589-591.
  • Nisbett, R. E. Hunger, obesity, and the ventromedial hypothalamus. Psychological Re­view, 1972, 79, 433-453.
  • Novin, D., Wyrwicka, W., & Bray, G. A. (Eds.) Hunger: basic mechanisms and clinical implications. New York: Raven Press, 1976.

O

  • Olton, D. S. Characteristics of spatial memory. In S. H. Hulse, H. Fowler, & W. K. Honig (Eds.), Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1978.
  • Olton, D. S., & Samuelson, R. J. Remembrance of places passed: spatial memory in rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1976, 2, 97-116.
  • Osgood, C. E. Method and theory in experimental psychology. New York: Oxford University Press, 1953.
  • Oster, J. F., & Wilson, E.O. Caste and ecology in the social insects. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1978.

P

  • Panksepp, J. Hypothalamic regulation of energy balance and feeding behavior. Federa­tion Proceedings, 1974, 33, 1150-1165.
  • Parker, G. A., & Stuart, R. A. Animal behavior as a strategy optimizer: evolution of resource assessment strategies and optimal emigration thresholds. American Naturalist, 1976, 110, 1055-1076.
  • Pavlov, I. P. Conditioned reflexes (G. V. Anrep, trans.). London: Oxford University Press, 1927.
  • Pearce, J. M., & Hall, G. A model for Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review, 1980, 87,532-552.
  • Peck, J. W. Situational determinants of the body weights defended by normal rats and rats with hypothalamic lesions. In D. Novin, W. Wyrwicka, & G. A. Bray (Eds.), Hunger: basic mechanisms and clinical implications. New York: Raven Press, 1976.
  • Peden, B. F., Browne, M. P., & Hearst, E. Persistent approaches to a signal for food despite food omission for approaching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1977, 3, 377-399.
  • Penney, J., & Schull, J. Functional differentiation of adjunctive drinking and wheel running in rats. Animal Learning and Behavior, 1977, 5, 272-280.
  • Pietrewicz, A. T., & Karnil, A. C. Search image formation in the blue jay (Cyanocitta cristata). Science, 1979, 204, 1332-1333.
  • Pinker, S. The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature. New York: Viking, 2002.
  • Pittendrigh, C. S. Adaptation, natural selection, and behavior. In A. Roe & G. G. Simpson (Eds.), Behavior and evolution. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1958.
  • Popper, K. The philosophy of Karl Popper. In P. Schilpp (Ed.), The library of living philosophers (Vol. 14). LaSalle, IL: Open Court, 1974.
  • Popper, K. R., & Eccles, J. C. The self and its brain. Berlin, W. Germany: Springer, 1977.
  • Posner, M. I. Chronometric explorations of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1978.
  • Posner, M. I., & Shulman, G. L. Cognitive science. In E. Hearst (Ed.), The first century of experimental psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1979.
  • Postman, L. The history and present status of the law of effect. Psychological Bulletin, 1947, 44, 489-563.
  • Poulton, E. B. Proof of the protective value of dimorphism in larvae. Transactions of the Entomological Society, London, 1888, 9, 50-56.
  • Poulton, E. C. The new psychophysics: six models for magnitude estimation. Psycholog­ical Bulletin, 1968, 69, 1-19.
  • Powers, W. T. Quantitative analysis of purposive systems: some spadework on the foundations of scientific psychology. Psychological Review, 1978, 85, 4l7-435.
  • Powley, T. L., & Opsahl, C. A. Autonomic components of the hypothalamic feeding syndromes. In D. Novin, W. Wyrwicka, & G. A. Bray (Eds.), Hunger: basic mechanisms and clinical implications. New York: Raven Press, 1976.
  • Premack, D. Reinforcement theory. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motiva­tion (Vol. 13). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1965.
  • Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. Increased eating in rats deprived of running. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1963, 6, 209-212.
  • Pulliam, H. R. Diet optimization with nutrient constraints. American Naturalist, 1975, 109, 765-768.
  • Pyke, G. H., Pulliam, H. R., & Charnov, E. L. Optimal foraging: a selective review of theory and tests. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 1977, 52, 137-154.

R

  • Rabbitt, P. Sorting, categorization, and visual search. In E. C. Carterette & M. P. Friedman (Eds.), Handbook of perception, Vol. XI. New York: Academic Press, 1978.
  • Rachlin, H. Introduction to modern behaviorism. San Francisco: Freeman, 1970.
  • Rachlin, H. Contrast and matching. Psychological Review, 1973, 80, 217-234.
  • Rachlin, H. A molar theory of reinforcement schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1978, 30, 345-360.
  • Rachlin, H., & Burkhard, B. The temporal triangle: response substitution in instrumental conditioning. Psychological Review, 1978, 85, 22-48.
  • Rachlin, H., & Green, L. Commitment, choice and self-control. Journal of the Experi­mental Analysis of Behavior, 1972, 17, 15-22.
  • Rachlin, H., Green, L., Kagel, J. H., & Battalio, R. C. Economic demand theory and psychological studies of choice. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 10). New York: Academic Press, 1976.
  • Ranson, S. W., & Clark, S. L. The anatomy of the nervous system. Philadelphia: Saunders, 1953. (Reprinted, 1958.)
  • Ratliff, F. Mach bands: quantitative studies on neural networks in the retina. San Francisco: Holden-Day, 1965.
  • Ratliff, F. Studies in excitation and inhibition in the retina: a collection of papers from the laboratories of H. K. Hartline. New York: Rockefeller University Press, 1974.
  • Real, L. A. The kinetics of functional response. American Naturalist, 1977, 111, 289-300.
  • Real, L. A. On uncertainty and the law of diminishing returns in evolution and behavior. In J. E. R. Staddon (Ed.), Limits to action: the allocation of individual behavior. New York: Academic Press, 1980.
  • Alliston K. Reid:
    • Reid, A. K., & Dale, R. H. I. Dynamic effects of food magnitude on interim-terminal interactions. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1983, 39, 135-148.
    • Reid, A. K., & Staddon, J. E. R. Schedule-induced drinking: elicitation, anticipation, or behavioral interaction? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1982, 38, 1-18.
  • Rescorla, R. A. Pavlovian conditioning and its proper control procedures. Psychological Review, 1967, 74, 71-80.
  • Rescorla, R. A. Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear condition­ing. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1968, 66, 1-5.
  • Rescorla, R. A. Pavlovian excitatory and inhibitory conditioning. In W. K. Estes (Ed.), Handbook of learning and cognitive processes (Vol. 2). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1975.
  • Rescorla, R. A. Stimulus generalization: some predictions from a model of Pavlovian conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1976, 2, 88-96.
  • Rescorla, R. A. Behavioral studies of Pavlovian conditioning. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 1988, 11, 329-352.
  • Rescorla, R. A., & Skucy, J. C. Effect of response-independent reinforcers during extinction. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1969, 67, 381-389.
  • Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A. Black & W. R. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1972.
  • Revusky, S. H. The role of interference in association over delay. In W. K. Honig & P. H. R. James (Eds.), Animal memory. New York: Academic Press, 1971.
  • Revusky, S. H. Learning as a general process with emphasis on data from feeding experiments. In N. W. Milgram, L. Krames, & T. M. Alloway (Eds.), Food aver­sion learning. New York: Plenum, 1977.
  • Reynolds, G. S. Attention in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behav­ior, 1961, 4, 203-208. (a)
  • Reynolds, G. S. Behavioral contrast. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1961, 4, 57-71. (b)
  • Reynolds, G. S. On some determinants of choice in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1963, 6, 53-59. (a)
  • Reynolds, G. S. Some limitations on behavioral contrast and induction during successive discrimination. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1963, 6, 131-139. (b)
  • Richards, J. J., & Blackman, D. E. Effect of time-out duration after food reinforcement in a fixed-ratio schedule with pigeons. Behaviour Analysis Letters, 1981, 1, 81-87.
  • Richardson, M. W. Multidimensional psychophysics. Psychological Bulletin, 1938, 35, 659-660.
  • Richelle, M. B. F. Skinner, a reappraisal. Hove, UK: Erlbaum, 1995.
  • Richelle, M., & Lejeune, H. Time in animal behaviour. Oxford: Pergamon, 1980.
  • Riley, D. A., & Roitblat, H. L. Selective attention and related processes in pigeons. In S. H. Hulse, H. Fowler, & W. K. Honig (Eds.), Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1978.
  • Rilling, M. Stimulus control and inhibitory processes. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.
  • Roberts, W. A. Spatial separation and visual differentiation of cues as factors influencing short-term memory in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychol­ogy, 1972, 78, 281-291.
  • Roberts, W. A., & Dale, R. H. I. Remembrance of places lasts: proactive inhibition and patterns of choice in rat spatial memory. Learning and Motivation, 1981, 12, 261-281.
  • Roberts, W. A., & Grant, D. S. Short-term memory in the pigeon with presentation time precisely controlled. Learning and Motivation, 1974, 5, 393-408.
  • Rodgers, W., & Rozin, P. Novel food preferences in thiamine-deficient rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1966, 61, l-4.
  • Roitblat, H. L. The meaning of representation in animal memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1982, 5, 353-406.
  • Roper, T. J. What is meant by the term “schedule-induced," and how general is schedule induction? Animal Learning and Behavior, 1981, 9, 433-440.
  • Rothkopf, E. Z. A measure of stimulus similarity and errors in some paired-associate learning tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1957, 53, 94-l0l.
  • Rosenblueth, A., Wiener, N., & Bigelow, J. Behavior, purpose and teleology. Philoso­phy of Science, 1943, 10, 18-24.
  • Rozin, P., & Kalat, J. W. Specific hungers and poison avoidance as adaptive specializa­tions of learning. Psychological Review, 1971, 78, 459-486.
  • Russek, M. A conceptual equation of intake control. In D. Novin, W. Wyrwicka, & G. A. Bray (Eds.), Hunger: basic mechanisms and clinical implications. New York: Raven Press, 1976.
  • Bertrand Russell:
    • Russell, B. History of western philosophy. London: Allen & Unwin, 1946.
    • Russell, B. The basic writings of Bertrand Russell. In R. E. Egner & L. E. Denonn (Eds.), New York: Simon & Schuster, 1961.

S

  • Sainsbury, R. S. Effect of proximity of elements on the feature-positive effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1971, 16, 315-325.
  • Samuelson, P. A. Foundations of economic analysis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni­versity Press, 1965.
  • Schachter, S., & Rodin, J. Obese humans and rats. Potomac, MD: Erlbaum, 1974.
  • Schick, K. Operants. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1971, 15, 413-423.
  • Schneider, B. A. A two-state analysis of fixed-interval responding in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1969, 12, 677-687.
  • Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. Controlled and automatic human information pro­cessing: I. detection, search and attention. Psychological Review, 1977, 84, 1-66.
  • Schoener, T. W. Theory of feeding strategies. Annual Review of Ecology and Systemat­ics, 1971, 2, 369-404.
  • Schutz, F. Prägung des sexual verhaltens von enten und gansen durch sozialeindrticke wirhend derjugendphase. Journal of Neurovisceral Relations, Supplementum, 1971, 10, 399-457.
  • Schwartz, B., & Gamzu, E. Pavlovian control of operant behavior. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.
  • Sclafani, A., & Springer, D. Dietary obesity in adult rats: similarities to hypothalamic and human obesity syndromes. Physiology and Behavior, 1976, 17, 461-471.
  • Sclafani, A., Springer, D., & Kluge, L. Effects of quinine adulterated diets on the food intake and body weight of obese and non-obese hypothalamic hyperphagic rats. Physiology and Behavior, 1976, 16, 631-640.
  • Seligman, M. E. P., & Hager, J. L. (Eds.) Biological boundaries of learning. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1972.
  • Seligman, M. E. P., & Maier, S. F. Failure to escape traumatic shock. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1967, 74, 1-9.
  • Seward, J. P. An experimental analysis of latent learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1949, 39,177-186.
  • Shannon, C., & Weaver, W. The mathematical theory of communication. Urbana: Univer­sity of Illinois Press, 1949.
  • Shaw, G. B. The black girl in search of God: and some lesser tales. Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin, 1946. (Reprinted from The adventures of the black girl in her search for God, first published in 1932.)
  • Shepard, R. N. Analysis of proximities as a technique for the study of information processing in man. Human Factors, 1963, 5, 33-48.
  • Shepard, R. N. Attention and the metric structure of the stimulus space. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 1964, 1, 54-87.
  • Shepard, R. N. Approximation to uniform gradients of generalization by monotone transformations of scale. In D. I. Mostofsky (Ed.), Stimulus generalization. Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1965.
  • Shepard, R. N. Farm, formation, and transformation of internal representations. In R. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: the Loyola symposium. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1975.
  • Shepard, R. N. Multidimensional scaling, tree-fitting, and clustering. Science, 1980, 210, 390-398.
  • Shepard, R. N., & Cooper, L. A. Mental images and their rotations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1982.
  • Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, B. Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 1971, 171, 701-703.
  • Sherrington, C. S. The integrative action of the nervous system. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1906. (Reprinted, 1947.)
  • Shettleworth, S. J. Constraints on learning. In D. S. Lehrman, R. A. Hinde, & E. Shaw (Eds.), Advances in the study of behavior (Vol. IV). New York: Academic Press, 1972.
  • Shettleworth, S. J. Reinforcement and the organization of behavior in golden hamsters: hunger, environment and food reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Psychology,1975, 104, 56-87.
  • Shettleworth, S. J. Reinforcement and the organization of behavior in golden hamsters: punishment of three action patterns. Learning and Motivation, 1978, 9, 99-123.
  • Shettleworth, S. J.Cognition, evolution and behavior. New York: Oxford, 1998.
  • Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. Controlled and automatic human information process­ing: II. perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychological Review, 1977, 84, 127-190.
  • Shimp, C. P. Probabilistically reinforced choice behavior in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1966, 9, 443-455.
  • Shimp, C. P. Optimal behavior in free-operant experiments. Psychological Review, 1969, 76, 97-112.
  • Shimp, C. P., & Wheatley, K. L. Matching to relative reinforcement frequency in multiple schedules with a short component duration. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1971, 15, 205-210.
  • Sibly, R., & McFarland, D. On the fitness of behavior sequences. American Naturalist, 1976, 110, 601-617.
  • Sidman, M. Tactics of scientific research: evaluating experimental data in psychology. New York: Basic Books, 1960.
  • Silberberg, A., Hamilton, B., Ziriax, J. M., & Casey, J. The structure of choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1978, 4, 368-398.
  • Simon, H. A. A note on Jost's law and exponential forgetting. Psychometrika, 1966, 31, 505-506.
  • Simon, H. A. Models of thought. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1979.
  • B. F. Skinner:
    • Skinner, B. F. On the rate of formation of a conditioned reflex. Journal of General Psychology, 1932, 7, 274-286.
    • Skinner, B. F. The generic nature of the concepts of stimulus and response. Journal of General Psychology, 1935, 12, 40-65.
    • Skinner, B. F. The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1938.
    • Skinner, B. F. "Superstition" in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1948, 38, 168-172.
    • Skinner, B. F. Are theories of learning necessary? Psychological Review, 1950, 57, 193-216.
    • Skinner, B. F. The phylogeny and ontogeny of behavior. Science, 1966, 153, 1205-1213.
  • Snowdon, C. T. Motivation, regulation, and the control of meal parameters with oral and intragastric feeding. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1969, 69, 91-100.
  • Spalding, D. A. Instinct: with original observations on young animals. Macmillan's Magazine, 1873, 27, 282-293.
  • Spence, K. The differential response in animals to stimuli varying in a single dimension. Psychological Review, 1937, 44, 435-444.
  • Sperry, R. W. Mechanisms of neural maturation. In S. S. Stevens (Ed.), Handbook of experimental psychology. New York: Wiley, 1951.
  • Sperry, R. W. A modified concept of consciousness. Psychological Review, 1969, 76, 532-536.
  • Squires, N., & Fantino, E. A model for choice in simple concurrent and concurrent-chains schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1971, 15, 27-38.
  • Squires, N., Norborg, J., & Fantino, E. Second-order schedules: discrimination of com­ponents. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1975, 24, 157-171.
  • J.E.R. Staddon:
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Some properties of spaced responding in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1965, 8, 19-27.
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Asymptotic behavior: the concept of the operant. Psychological Review, 1967, 74, 377-391.
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Multiple fixed-interval schedules: transient contrast and temporal inhibition. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1969, 12, 583-590.
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Temporal effects of reinforcement: a negative "frustration" effect. Learning and Motivation, 1970, 1, 227-247. (a)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Effect of reinforcement duration on fixed-interval responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1970, 13, 9-11. (b)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Reinforcement omission on temporal go-no-go schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1972, 18, 223-229. (a)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Temporal control and the theory of reinforcement schedules. In R. M. Gilbert & J. R. Millenson (Eds.), Reinforcement: behavioral analyses. New York: Academic Press, 1972. (b)
    • Staddon, J. E R. On the notion of cause, with applications to behaviorism. Behaviorism, 1973, 1, 25-63.
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Temporal control, attention and memory. Psychological Review, 1974, 81, 375-391.
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Limitations on temporal control: generalization and the effects of context. British Journal of Psychology, 1975, 66, 229-246. (a)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. A note on the evolutionary significance of supernormal stimuli. Ameri­can Naturalist, 1975, 109, 541-545. (b)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Schedule-induced behavior. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977. (a)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Behavioral competition in conditioning situations: notes toward a theory of generalization and inhibition. In H. Davis & H. M. B. Hurwitz (Eds.), Operant-Pavlovian interactions. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1977. (b)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Theory of behavioral power functions. Psychological Review, 1978, 85, 305-320.
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Operant behavior as adaptation to constraint. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1979, 108, 48-67. (a)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Regulation and time allocation: a commentary on “conservation in behavior." Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1979, 108, 35-40. (b)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. (Ed.) Limits to action: the allocation of individual behavior. New York: Academic Press, 1980. (a)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Optimality analyses of operant behavior and their relation to optimal foraging. In J. E. R. Staddon (Ed.), Limits to action: the allocation of individual behavior. New York: Academic Press, 1980. (b)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. On a possible relation between cultural transmission and genetical evolution. In P. Klopfer & P. P. G. Bateson (Eds.), Perspectives in ethology. Vol.4: Advantages of diversity. London: Plenum, 1981. (a)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Cognition in animals: learning as program assembly. Cognition, 1981, 10, 287-294. (b)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Behavioral competition, contrast, and matching. In M. L. Commons, R. J. Herrnstein, & H. Rachlin (Eds.), Quantitative analyses of operant behavior: matching and maximizing accounts. Quantitative analyses of behavior, Vol.II (five-volume series). Cambridge, MA: Ballinger, 1982.
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Inference, memory and representation. In N.M. Weinberger, J.L. McGaugh, & G. Lynch (Eds.) Memory systems of the brain: animal and human cognitive processes. New York: Guilford Publications, 1985.
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Quasi-dynamic choice models: melioration and ratio-invariance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1988, 49, 303-320.
    • Staddon, J.E.R. The new behaviorism: mind, mechanism and society. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press, 2001. (a)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Adaptive dynamics: the theoretical analysis of behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT/Bradford, 2001. (b)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. (in press) Humanism and Skinner’s Radical Behaviorism. In Behavior Theory and Philosophy, Ed. Lattal & Chase.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Ayres, S. Sequential and temporal properties of behavior in­duced by a schedule of periodic food delivery. Behaviour, 1975, 54, 26-49.
    • Staddon, J. E. R. & Cerutti, D. T. Operant behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 2003, 54, 115-144.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Frank, J. Mechanisms of discrimination reversal. Animal Behaviour, 1974, 22, 802-828.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Gendron, R. P. Optimal detection of cryptic prey may lead to predator switching. American Naturalist, 1983, 122, 843-848.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Higa, J. J. Multiple time scales in simple habituation. Psychological Review, 1996, 103, 720-733.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Higa, J. J. Time and memory: towards a pacemaker-free theory of interval timing. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1999, 71, 215-251.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., Hinson, J. M., & Kram, R. Optimal choice. Journal of the Experimen­tal Analysis of Behavior, 1981, 35, 397-412.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Horner, J. M. Stochastic choice models: a comparison between Bush-Mosteller and a source-independent reward-following model. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1989, 52, 57-64.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Innis, N. K. Reinforcement omission on fixed-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1969, 12, 689-700.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Motheral, S. On matching and maximizing in operant choice experiments. Psychological Review, 1978, 85, 436-444.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Motheral, S. Response independence, matching, and maximizing: a reply to Heyman. Psychological Review, 1979, 86, 501-505.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Simmelhag, V. L. The "superstition" experiment: a reexamination of its implications for the principles of adaptive behavior. Psychological Review,1971, 78, 3-43.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Zanutto, B. S. Feeding dynamics: why rats eat in meals and what this means for foraging and feeding regulation. In M.E. Bouton & M.S. Fanselow (Eds.), Learning, motivation and cognition: the functional behaviorism of Robert C. Bolles. Washington: American Psychological Association, 1997.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Zhang, Y. On the assignment-of-credit problem in operant learning. In M. L. Commons, S. Grossberg, & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Neural networks of conditioning and action, the XIIth Harvard Symposium. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates, 1991.
  • Starr, B., & Staddon, J. E. R. Temporal control on fixed-interval schedules: signal-properties of reinforcement and blackout. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1974, 22, 535-545.
  • Stevens, S. S. On the averaging of data. Science, 1955, 121, 113-116.
  • Stigler, G. J. The development of utility theory. II. The Journal of Political Economy, 1950, 58, 373-396.
  • Stubbs, D. A. Second-order schedules and the problem of conditioned reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1971, 16, 289-313.
  • Stubbs, D. A. Response bias and the discrimination of stimulus duration. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1976, 25, 243-250.
  • Sutherland, N. S., & Mackintosh, N. J. Mechanisms of animal discrimination learning. New York: Academic Press, 1971.
  • Suzuki, S., Augerinos, G., & Black, A. H. Stimulus control of spatial behavior on the eight-arm maze in rats. Learning and Motivation, 1980, 6, 77-81.
  • Swazey, J. P. Reflexes and motor integration: Sherrington's concept of integrative ac­tion. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1969.
  • Symposia of the Society for Experimental Biology, No. XVIII: Homeostasis and feedback mechanisms. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 1964.

T

  • Teghtsoonian, R. On the exponents in Stevens' law and the constants in Ekman's law. Psychological Review, 1971, 78, 71-80.
  • Teitelbaum, P., & Epstein, A. N. The lateral hypothalamic syndrome. Psychological Review, 1962, 69, 74-90.
  • Terrace, H. S. Stimulus control. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: areas of research and application. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1966.
  • Thielcke, G. A. Bird sounds. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press, 1976.
  • Thompson, D. W. On growth and form. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1961. (Abridgment of the 1917 and 1942 editions, edited by J. T. Bonner.)
  • Thompson, R. F., & Spencer, W. A. Habituation: a model phenomenon for the study of neuronal substrates of behavior. Psychological Review, 1966, 73, 1643.
  • Thorndike, E. L. Animal intelligence: an experimental study of the associative processes in animals. Psychological Monographs, 1898, 2, 109.
  • Thorndike, E. L. Animal intelligence. New York: Macmillan, 1911.
  • Timberlake, W. A molar equilibrium theory of learned performance. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 14). New York: Academic Press, 1980.
  • Timberlake, W. Biological behaviorism. In W. O’Donohue & R. Kitchener (Eds.), Handbook of Behaviorism. San Diego: Academic Press, 1999.
  • Timberlake, W., & Allison, J. Response deprivation: an empirical approach to instru­mental performance. Psychological Review, 1974, 8, 146-164.
  • Tinbergen, L. The natural control of insects in pinewoods: I. factors affecting the inten­sity of predation by songbirds. Archives Neerlandaises de Zoologie, 1960, 13, 265-343.
  • Tinbergen, N. Social releasers and the experimental method required for their study. Wilson Bulletin, 1948, 60, 6-51.
  • Tinbergen, N. The study of instinct. New York: Oxford University Press, 1951.
  • Tinbergen, N. Curious naturalists. New York: Basic Books, 1958.
  • Tinbergen, N. On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 1963, 20, 410-433.
  • Tinbergen, N., Impekoven, M., & Franck, D. An experiment on spacing out as a defense against predators. Behaviour, 1967, 28, 307-321.
  • Toates, F. Control theory in biology and experimental psychology. London: Hutchinson Educational, 1975.
  • Toates, F. M., & Archer, J. A comparative review of motivational systems using classical control theory. Animal Behaviour, 1978, 26, 368-380.
  • Toates, F. M., & Halliday, T. R. (Eds.) The analysis of motivational processes. London: Academic Press, 1980.
  • Todorov, J. C. Component duration and relative response rates in multiple schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1972, 17, 45-49.
  • Todorov, J. C. Interaction of frequency and magnitude of reinforcement on concurrent performances. Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 1973, 19, 451-458.
  • Tolman, E. C. Purposive behavior in animals and men. New York: Appleton-Century­-Crofts, 1932. (Reprinted, University of California Press, 1949.)
  • Tolman, E. C., & Honzik, C. H. Introduction and removal of reward, and maze perfor­mance in rats. University of California Publications in Psychology, 1930, 4, 257-275.
  • Torgerson, W. S. Multidimensional scaling: theory and method. Psychometrika, 1952, 17, 401-419.
  • Toulmin, S. Neuroscience and human understanding. In G. Quarton, F. Schmitt, & E. Melnechuk (Eds.), Neuroscience: a study program. New York: Rockefeller Univer­sity Press, 1967

U-V

  • Uexküll, J. von. Streifzuge durch die Umwelten von Tieren undMenschen. Berlin: Springer,1934.
  • Uhl, C. N. Response elimination in rats with schedules of omission training, including yoked and response-independent reinforcement comparisons. Learning and Motivation, 1974, 5, 511-531
  • Verplanck, W. S. Since learned behavior is innate, and vice versa, what now? Psychological Review, 1955, 52, 139-144.
  • Von Neuman, J., & Morgenstern, O. Theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1947.

W

  • Waddington, C. H. Genetic assimilation of the Bithorax phenotype. Evolution, 1956, 10 ,1-13.
  • Waddington, C. H. Evolutionary adaptation. In S. Tax (Ed.), Evolution after Darwin. Vol.1: The evolution of life. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1960.
  • Waddington, C. H. New patterns in genetics and development. London: Allen & Unwin, 1962.
  • Wagner, A. R. Expectancies and the priming of STM. In S. H. Hulse, H. Fowler, & W. K. Honig (Eds.), Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1978.
  • Wagner, A. R., Rudy, J. W., & Whitlow, J. W. Rehearsal in animal conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1973, 97, 407-426.
  • Wagner, A. R., & Terry, W. S. Backward conditioning to a CS following an expected vs. a surprising UCS. Animal Learning and Behavior, 1975, 3, 370-374.
  • Wallsten, T. S. (Ed.) Cognitive processes in choice and decision behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1980.
  • Walsh, V. C. Introduction to contemporary microeconomics. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1970.
  • Watson, J. B. Psychology from the standpoint of a behaviorist. Philadelphia: Lippincott, 1919.
  • Werner, E. E., & Hall, D. J. Optimal foraging and the size selection of prey by the Bluegill Sunfish (Lepomis macrochirus). Ecology, 1974, 55, 1216-1232.
  • Wessells, M. G. The effects of reinforcement upon the pre-pecking behaviors of pigeons in the autoshaping experiment. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1974, 21, 125-144.
  • West, M. J., King, A. P., Eastzer, D. H., & Staddon, J. E. R. A bioassay of isolate cowbird song. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1979, 93, l24-l33.
  • Whipple, W. R., & Fantino, E. Key-peck durations under behavioral contrast and differ­ential reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1980, 34, 167-176.
  • White, K. G. Behavioral contrast as differential time allocation. Journal of the Experi­mental Analysis of Behavior, 1978, 29, 151-160.
  • Whitham, T. G. Coevolution of foraging in Bombus and nectar dispensing in Cilopsis: a last dreg theory. Science, 1977, 197, 593-596.
  • Wickens, D. D., & Wickens, C. D. Some factors related to pseudoconditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1942, 31, 518-526.
  • Wilcoxon, H. C. Historical introduction to the problem of reinforcement. In J. C. Tapp (Ed.), Reinforcement and behavior. New York: Academic Press, 1969.
  • Williams, B. A. Reinforcement, choice, and response strength. In R. C. Atkinson, R. J. Herrnstein, G. Lindzey, & R. D. Luce (Eds.), Stevens' handbook of experimental psychology (2 ed.,). New York: John Wiley, 1988.
  • Williams, D. R. Classical conditioning and incentive motivation. In W. F. Prokasy (Ed.), Classical conditioning: a symposium. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1965.
  • Williams, D. R., & Williams, H. Auto-maintenance in the pigeon: sustained pecking despite contingent non-reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Be­havior, 1969, 12, 511-520.
  • Wilson, M. P. Periodic reinforcement interval and number of periodic reinforcements as parameters of response strength. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1954, 47, 51-56.
  • Wirtshafter, D., & Davis, J. D. Set points, settling points, and the control of body weight. Physiology and Behavior, 1977, 19, 75-78.
  • Wynne, C. D. L., & Staddon, J. E. R. Typical delay determines waiting time on periodic-food schedules: static and dynamic tests. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1988, 50, 197-210.

Y-Z

  • Yates, F. A. The art of memory. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1966.
  • Zeiler, M. D., & Buchman, I. B. Response requirements as constraints on output. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1979, 32, 29-49.
  • Zeiler, M. D., & Harzem, P. (Eds.) Reinforcement and the organization of behavior. New York: Wiley, 1979.
  • Zener, K. The significance of behavior accompanying conditioned salivary secretion for theories of the conditioned response. American Journal of Psychology, 1937, 50, 384-403.
  • Zippelius, H. Die karawanenbildung bei feld- und hausspitzmaus. Zeitschrifi für Tierpsychologie, 1972, 30, 305-320.
  • Ziriax, J. M., & Silberberg, A. Discrimination and emission of different key-peck dura­tions in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Pro­cesses, 1978, 4, 1-21.