Adaptive Behavior and Learning/contents/References

from HTYP, the free directory anyone can edit if they can prove to me that they're not a spambot
Jump to navigation Jump to search

Title Page
Preface
Chapter 1
Chapter 2
Chapter 3
Chapter 4
Chapter 5
Chapter 6
Chapter 7
Chapter 8
Chapter 9
Chapter 10
Chapter 11
Chapter 12
Chapter 13
Chapter 14
Chapter 15
Chapter 16
References

This article is in need of editing attention:
"Smart" quotes and other special characters did not transcribe properly.

A

  • Allison, J. Paired baseline performance as a behavioral ideal. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1981, 35, 355-366.
  • Allison, J., Miller, M., & Wozny, M. Conservation in behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1979, 108, 4-34.
  • Alloy, L. B., & Seligman, M. E. P. On the cognitive component of learned helplessness and depression. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 13). New York: Academic Press, 1979.
  • Anderson, M. C., & Shettleworth, S. J. Behavioral adaptation to fixed-time food delivery in golden hamsters. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1977, 25, 33-49.
  • Arend, L. E., Buehler, J. N., & Lockhead, G. R. Difference information in brightness perception. Perception and Psychophysics, 1971, 9, 367-370.
  • Ashby, W. R. An introduction to cybernetics. London: Chapman & Hall, 1956.
  • Atkinson, J. W., & Birch, D. The dynamics of action. New York: Wiley, 1970.
  • Attneave, F. Dimensions of similarity. American Journal of Psychology, 1950, 63, 516-556.
  • Autor, S. M. The strength of conditioned reinforcers as a function of frequency and probability of reinforcement. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, 1960.
  • Azrin, N.H., Hutchinson, R. R., & Hake, D. F. Attack, avoidance, and escape reactions to aversive shock. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1967, 10, 131-138.

B

  • Bacotti, A. V. Matching under concurrent fixed-ratio variable-interval schedules of food presentation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1977, 25, 171-182.
  • Barrera, F. J. Centrifugal selection of signal-directed pecking. Journal of the Experimen­tal Analysis of Behavior, 1974, 22, 341-355.
  • Bateson, P. P. G. The imprinting of birds. In S.A. Barnett (Ed.), Ethology and develop­ment. London: Heinemann, 1974.
  • Baum, W. M. Time-based and count-based measurement of preference. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1976, 26, 27-35.
  • Baum, W. M. Understanding behaviorism: science, behavior and culture. New York: HarperCollins, 1994.
  • Baumol, W. J. Economic theory and operations analysis (4th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.
  • Beatty, W. W., & Shavalia, D. A. Rat spatial memory: resistance to retroactive interfer­ence at long retention intervals. Animal Learning and Behavior, 1980, 8, 550-552.
  • Békésy, G. von. Sensory inhibition. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1967.
  • Bell, P. R. (Ed.) Darwin's biological work. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1959.
  • Benson, W. W. Evidence for the evolution of unpalatability through kin selection in the Heliconiinae (Lepidoptera). American Naturalist, 1971, 105, 213-226.
  • Bitterman, M. E. Phyletic differences in learning. American Psychologist, 1965, 20, 396-410.
  • Bitterman, M. E. Habit-reversal and probability learning: rats, birds, and fish. In R. M. Gilbert & N. S. Sutherland (Eds.), Animal discrimination learning. New York: Academic Press, 1969.
  • Donald Blough:
    • Blough, D. S. Steady-state data and a quantitative model of operant generalization and discrimination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1975, 104, 3-21.
    • Blough, D. S. Effects of the number and form of stimuli on visual search in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1979, 5, 211-223.
  • Boakes, R. From Darwin to behaviourism: psychology and the minds of animals. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1984.
  • Bogdany, F. J. Linkage of learning signals in honey bee orientation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 1978, 3, 323-336.
  • Bolles, R. C. Avoidance and escape learning: simultaneous acquisition of different responses. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1969, 68, 355-358.
  • Bolles, R. C. Species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning. Psychological Review, 1970, 77, 32-48.
  • Boren, J. J. Resistance to extinction as a function of the fixed ratio. Journal of Experimen­tal Psychology, 1961, 61, 304-308.
  • Boring, E. G. Sensation and perception in the history of experimental psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1942.
  • Boring, E. G. A history of experimental psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1957.
  • Bouzas, A., & Baum, W. M. Behavioral contrast of time allocation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1976, 25, 179-184.
  • Bower, G. H. & Hilgard, E. R. Theories of learning (5th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1981.
  • Brandt, H. Die lichtorientierung der mehimotte, Ephestia kuehniella. Zeitschrift fur Vergleichende Physiologie, 1934, 20, 646-673.
  • Brazier, M. B. The historical development of neurophysiology. In J. Field, H. Magoun, & V. E. Hall (Eds.), Handbook of physiology Section I. Washington, DC: American Physiological Society, 1959.
  • Breland, K., & Breland, M. The misbehavior of organisms. American Psychologist, 1961, 16, 661-664.
  • Breland, K., & Breland, M. Animal behavior. New York: Macmillan, 1966.
  • Brobeck, J. R. Effects of variations in activity, food intake, and environmental tempera­ture on weight gain in the albino rat. American Journal of Physiology, 1945, 143, 1-5.
  • Brown, P. L., & Jenkins, H. M. Auto-shaping of the pigeon's key-peck. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1968, 11, 1-8.
  • Buck, S. L., Rothstein, B., & Williams, B. A. A re-examination of local contrast in multiple schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1975, 24, 291-301.
  • Buddenbrock, W. von. Mechanismus der phototropen bewegungen. Wissenschafi Meeresuntersuch. N. F. Abteilung Helgoland, 1922, 15, 1-19.
  • Bush, R. R., & Mosteller, F. Stochastic models for learning. New York: Wiley, 1955.
  • Buszâki, G. The "where is it?" reflex: autoshaping the orienting response. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1982, 37, 46l-484.
  • Buszâki, G., Grastyán, B., Winiczai, Z., & Mód, L. Maintenance of signal directed behavior in a response dependent paradigm: a systems approach. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 1979, 39, 201-217.

C

  • Campbell, D. T. On the conflicts between biological and social evolution and between psychology and moral tradition. American Psychologist, 1975, 30, 1103-1126.
  • Caplan, A. I., & Ordahl, C. P. Irreversible gene repression model for the control of development. Science, 1978, 201, 120-130.
  • Caraco, T., Martindale, S., & Whitham, T. S. An empirical demonstration of risk-sensitive foraging preferences. Animal Behaviour, 1980, 28, 820-830.
  • Catania, A. C. Concurrent performances: reinforcement interaction and response inde­pendence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1963, 6, 253-264.
  • Catania, A. C. Self-inhibiting effects of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1973, 19, 517-526.
  • Catania, A. C. Learning. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1979.
  • Catania, A. C. Preference for free choice over forced choice in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1980, 34, 77-86. (a)
  • Catania, A. C. Freedom of choice: a behavioral analysis. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 14). New York: Academic Press, 1980. (b)
  • Catania, A. C. Contingency, contiguity, correlation, and the concept of causation. In P. Harzem & M. D. Zeiler (Eds.), Advances in the study of behaviour. Vol .2: Predictability, correlation and contiguity. Chichester, UK: John Wiley, 1981.
  • Catania, A. C., & Gill, C. A. Inhibition and behavioral contrast. Psychonomic Science, 1964, 1, 257-258.
  • Catania, A. C., & Reynolds, G. S. A quantitative analysis of the behavior maintained by interval schedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1968, 11, 327-383.
  • Catania, A. C., Silverman, P. J., & Stubbs, D. Concurrent performances: stimulus-control gradients during schedules of signalled and unsignalled concurrent reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1974, 21, 99-107.
  • Catania, A.C., Yohalem, R., & Silverman, P.J. Contingency and stimulus change in chained schedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1980, 5, 167-173.
  • Catchpole, C. K. Vocal communication in birds. Baltimore: University Park Press, 1979.
  • Cerella, J. Visual classes and natural categories in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1979, 5, 68-77.
  • Charman, l., & Davison, m. On the effects of food deprivation and component reinforcer rates on multiple-schedule performance. Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 1983, 40, 239-251.
  • Charnov, E. L. Optimal foraging: the marginal value theorem. Theoretical Population Biology, 1976, 9, 129-136.
  • Cherry, C. On human communication. New York: Science Editions, 1961.
  • Chiang, A. C. Fundamental methods of mathematical economics. Tokyo: McGraw-Hill Kogakusha, 1974.
  • Chomsky, N. A review of B. F. Skinner's Verbal behavior. Language, 1959, 35, 26-58.
  • Russell M. Church:
    • Church, R. M. Systematic effect of random error in the yoked control design. Psycholog­ical Bulletin, 1964, 62, 122-131.
    • Church, R. M. The internal clock. In S. H. Hulse, H. Fowler, & W. K. Honig (Eds.), Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1978.
    • Church, R. M. Short-term memory for time intervals. Learning and Motivation, 1980, 11 , 208-219.
  • Collier, G., Hirsch, E., & Hamlin, P. H. The ecological determinants of reinforcement in the rat. Physiology and Behavior, 1972, 9, 705-716.
  • Collier, G., Hirsch, E., & Kanarek, R. The operant revisited. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), The handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.
  • Cook, R. G. (Ed.) Avian visual cognition. Cyberbook: http://www.pigeon.psy.tufts.edu/avc/toc.htm, 2001.
  • Cook, R. M., & Cockrell, B. J. Predator ingestion rate and its bearing on feeding time and the theory of optimal diets. Journal of Animal Ecology, 1978, 47, 529-547.
  • Cooper, L. A. Internal representation. In D. R. Griffin, (Ed.) Animal mind- human mind. Berlin/Heidelberg/New York: Springer-Verlag, 1982.
  • Corning, W., Dyal, J., & Willows, A. O. D. (Eds.) Invertebrate learning (Vols. 1-3). New York: Plenum, 1973.
  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.) The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and generation of culture. New York: Oxford University Press, 1992.
  • Cowie, R. J. Optimal foraging in Great Tits (Parus major). Nature, 1977, 268, 137-139.
  • Crespi, L. P. Quantitative variation in incentive and performance in the white rat. Ameri­can Journal of Psychology, 1942, 5, 467-517.
  • Crovitz, H. F. Galton's Walk: methods for the analysis of thinking, intelligence, and creativity. New York: Harper & Row, 1970.
  • Croze, H. Searching image in carrion crows. Berlin: Paul Parey, 1970.
  • Crozier, W. J., & Cole, W. H. The phototropic excitation of Limax. Journal of General Phvsiology, 1929, 12, 669-674.

D

  • Dale, R. H. I., & Staddon, J. E. R. A temporal theory of spatial memory. Unpublished.
  • D'Amato, M. R. Delayed matching and short-term memory in monkeys. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: advances in research and theory. New York: Academic Press, 1973.
  • Charles R. Darwin:
    • Darwin, C. The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. London: John Murray, 1871.
    • Darwin, C. The origin of species. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1951. (Reprinted from the 6th ed., 1872.)
    • Darwin, C. The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London: John Murray,1872.
    • Darwin, C. The variation of animals and plants under domestication. London: John Murray, 1875. (a)
    • Darwin, C. The movements and habits of climbing plants. London: John Murray, 1875. (b)
    • Darwin, C. The power of movement in plants. London: John Murray, 1880.
  • Davey, M.P., Srinivasan, M.V., & Maddess T. The Craik-O’Brien-Cornsweet illusion in honeybees. Naturwissenschaften, 1998, 85, 73-75.
  • Davies, N. B. Prey selection and the search strategy of the spotted flycatcher (Muscicapa striata), a field study on optimal foraging. Animal Behaviour, 1977, 25, 1016-1033.
  • Davies, N. B., & Houston, A. I. Owners and satellites: the economics of territory defence in the Pied Wagtail, Motacilla alba. Journal of Animal Ecology, 1981, 50, 157-180.
  • Davis, J. D., & Levine, M. W. A model for the control of ingestion. Psychological Review, 1977, 84, 379-4l2.
  • Davis, M. Socially induced flight reactions in pigeons. Animal Behaviour, 1975, 23, 597-601.
  • Dawkins, M. Perceptual changes in chicks, another look at the “search image" concept. Animal Behaviour, 1971, 19, 566-574. (a)
  • Dawkins, M. Shifts of "attention" in chicks during feeding. Animal Behaviour, 1971, 19, 575-582. (b)
  • Dennett, D. C. Brainstorms. Montgomery, VT: Bradford, 1978.
  • de Ruiter, L. Some experiments on the camouflage of stick caterpillars. Behaviour, 1952, 4, 222-232.
  • de Villiers, P. A. The law of effect and avoidance: a quantitative relationship between response rate and shock-frequency reduction. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1974, 21, 223-235.
  • de Villiers, P. A. Choice in concurrent schedules and a quantitative formulation of the Law of Effect. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.
  • de Villiers, P. A., & Herrnstein, R. J. Toward a law of response strength. Psychological Bulletin, 1976, 83, 1131-1153.
  • Dickinson, A. Contemporary animal learning theory. Cambridge, UK/New York: Cambridge University Press, 1980.
  • Dickinson, A., Hall, G., & Mackintosh, N. J. Surprise and the attenuation of blocking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1976, 2, 213-222.
  • Dickinson, A., & Mackintosh, N.J. Classical conditioning in animals. Annual Review of Psychology, 1978, 29, 587-612.
  • Domjan, M. Ingestional aversion learning: unique and general processes. In J. S. Rosenblatt, R. A. Hinde, & M. C. Busnel (Eds.), Advances in the study of behavior (Vol. 11). New York: Academic Press, 1980.
  • Dunham, P. J. Punishment: method and theory. Psychological Review, 1971, 78, 58-70.
  • Dunham, P. J. The nature of reinforcing stimuli. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.

E

  • Edelman, G. Neural Darwinism: the theory of neuronal group selection. New York: Basic Books, 1987.
  • Edmon, E. L. Multiple schedule component duration: a reanalysis of Shimp and Wheatley (1971) and Todorov (1972). Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1978, 30, 239-241.
  • Einhorn, H. J., & Hogarth, R. M. Confidence in judgment: persistence of the illusion of validity. Psychological Review, 1978, 85, 395-416.
  • Ekman, G. Dimensions of color vision. Journal of Psychology, 1954, 38, 467-474.
  • Elliffe, d., & Davison, m. Performance in continuously available multiple schedules. Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 1985, 44, 343-353.
  • Ellison, G. D., & Konorski, J. Separation of the salivary and motor responses in instru­mental conditioning. Science, 1964, 146, 1071-1072.
  • Estes, W. K. Effects of competing reactions on the conditioning curve for bar-pressing. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1950, 40, 200-205.
  • Estes, W. K. The problem of inference from curves based on group data. Psychological Bulletin, 1956, 53, 134-140.
  • Estes, W. K. The statistical approach to learning theory. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: a study of a science (Vol.2). New York: McGraw-Hill, 1959.
  • Estes, W. K., & Skinner, B. F. Some quantitative properties of anxiety. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1941, 29, 390-400.
  • Estes, W. K., Koch, S., MacCorquodale, K., Meehl, P. E., Mueller, C. G., Schoenfeld, W. N., & Verplanck, W. S. Modern learning theory. New York: Appleton-Century­-Crofts, 1954.
  • Ettinger, R. H., & Staddon, J. E. R. The operant regulation of feeding: a static analysis. Behavioral Neuroscience, 1983, 97, 639-653.

F

  • Falk, J. L. Production of polydipsia in normal rats by an intermittent food schedule. Science, 1961, 133, 195-196.
  • Fantino, E. Choice and rate of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1969,12, 723-730.
  • Fantino, E. Conditioned reinforcement. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs. NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.
  • Fantino, E. Contiguity, response strength, and the delay-reduction hypothesis. In P. Harzem & M.D. Zeiler (Eds.), Advances in the study of behaviour. Vol. 2: Predictability, correlation, and contiguity. Chichester, UK: Wiley, 1981.
  • Fantino, E., & Logan, C. A. The experimental analysis of behavior: a biological per­spective. San Francisco: Freeman, 1979.
  • Farley, J. Automaintenance, contrast, and contingencies: effects of local vs. overall and prior vs. impending reinforcement context. Learning and Motivation, 1980, 11, l9-48.
  • Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. Schedules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton-­Century-Crofts, 1957.
  • Fletcher, H. J. The delayed-response problem. In A. M. Schrier, H. F. Harlow, & F. Stollnitz (Eds.), Behavior of nonhuman primates. New York: Academic Press, 1965.
  • Ford, E. B. Polymorphism. Biological Review, 1945, 20, 73-88.
  • Fraenkel, G. S. Die mechanik der orientierung der tiere im raum. Biological Review, 1931, 6, 36-87.
  • Fraenkel, G. S., & Gunn, D. L. The orientation of animals. Fair Lawn, NJ: Oxford University Press, 1940. (Dover edition: 1961.)
  • Frank, J., & Staddon, J. E. R. The effects of restraint on temporal discrimination behav­ior. Psychological Record, 1974, 23, 123-130.
  • Friedman, M., & Savage, J. The utility analysis of choices involving risk. Journal of Political Economy, 1948, 56, 279-304.
  • Friedman, M. I., & Stricker, E.M. The physiological psychology of hunger: a physiolog­ical perspective. Psychological Review, 1976, 83, 409-431.

G

  • Gallistel, C. R. The organization of action: a new synthesis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1990.
  • Gallistel, C. R., & Gibbon, J. The symbolic foundations of conditioned behavior. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Associates, 2002.
  • Garcia, J. Tilting at the paper mills of academe. American Psychologist, 1981, 36, 149-158.
  • Garcia, J., Clarke, J., & Hankins, W. G. Natural responses to scheduled rewards. In P. P. G. Bateson & P. Klopfer (Eds.), Perspectives in ethology. New York: Plenum, 1973.
  • Garcia, J., Kimeldorf, D. J., & Koelling, R. A. A conditioned aversion towards saccharin resulting from exposure to gamma radiation. Science, 1955, 122, 157-159.
  • Gendron, R. P. Foraging for cryptic prey. Doctoral dissertation, Duke University, 1982.
  • Gendron, R. P., & Staddon, J. E. R. Searching for cryptic prey: the effect of search speed. American Naturalist, 1983, 121, 172-186.
  • Gibbon, J. Scalar expectancy and Weber's law in animal timing. Psychological Review, 1977, 84, 279-325.
  • Gibbon, J., Berryman, R., & Thompson, R. L. Contingency spaces and measures in classical conditioning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1974, 21, 585-605.
  • Gibbon, J., Locurto, C., & Terrace, H. Signal food contingency and signal food fre­quency in a continuous-trials autoshaping paradigm. Animal Learning and Behavior, 1975, 3, 313-324.
  • Gibbs, C. M., Latham, S. B., & Gormezano, I. Classical conditioning of the rabbit nictitating membrane response: effects of reinforcement schedule on response maintenance and resistance to extinction. Animal Learning and Behavior, 1978, 6, 209-215.
  • Gigerenzer, G., & Selten, R. (Eds.) Bounded rationality: The adaptive toolbox. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2002.
  • Gill, F. B., & Wolf, L. L. Economics of feeding territoriality in the golden-winged sunbird. Ecology, 1978, 56, 333-345.
  • Glaister, S. Mathematical methods for economists. London: Gray-Mills, 1972.
  • Glazer, H. I., & Weiss, J. M. Long-term interference effect: an alternative to "learned helplessness." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1976, 2, 201-213.
  • Gottlieb, G. Development of species identification in birds. Chicago: University of Chi­cago Press, 1971.
  • Gottlieb, G. On the acoustic basis of species identification in wood ducklings (Aix sponsa). Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1974, 87, 1038-1048.
  • Gould, J. L., & Gould, C. G. The animal mind. New York: W. H. Freeman, 1999.
  • Gould, S. J. Ontogeny and phylogeny. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1977.
  • grace, r. c. a contextual model of concurrent-chains choice. journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 1994, 61, 113-129.
  • Granit, R. Charles Scott Sherrington: an appraisal. London: Nelson, 1966.
  • Grant, D. S. Delayed alternation in the rat: effect of contextual stimuli on proactive interference. Learning and Motivation, 1980, 11, 339-354.
  • Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley, 1966.
  • Green, L., & Snyderman, M. Choice between rewards differing in amount and delay: toward a choice model of self control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1980, 34, 135-147.
  • Greenwood, M. R. C., Quartermain, D., Johnson, R. R., Cruce, J. A. F., & Hirsch, J. Food motivated behavior in genetically obese and hypothalamic-hyperphagic rats and mice. Physiology and Behavior, 1974, 13, 687-692.
  • Grindley, G. C. The formation of a simple habit in guinea pigs. British Journal of Psychology, 1932, 23, 127-147.
  • Grodins, F. S. Control theory and biological systems. New York: Columbia University Press, 1963.
  • Grossberg, S. Psychophysiological substrates of schedule interactions and behavioral contrast. SIAM-AMS Proceedings, 1981, 13, 157-186.
  • Grossberg, S. Studies of mind and brain. Boston: D. Reidel, 1982.
  • Guttman, N. Effects of discrimination formation on generalization. In D. I. Mostofsky (Ed.), Stimulus generalization. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press, 1965.
  • Guttman, N., & Kalish, H. I. Discriminability and stimulus generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1956, 51, 79-88.

H

  • Hall, G., & Pearce, J. M. Latent inhibition of a CS during CS-US pairings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1979, 5, 3l-42.
  • Hamilton, B. E., & Silberberg, A. Contrast and autoshaping in multiple schedules vary­ing reinforcer rate and duration. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1978, 30, 107-122.
  • Hanson, H. M. The effects of discrimination training on stimulus generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1959, 58, 321-334.
  • Harlow, H. F. The formation of learning sets. Psychological Review, 1949, 56, 51-65.
  • Harlow, H. F., & Harlow, M. K. The affectional systems. In A. M. Schrier, H. F. Harlow, & F. Stollnitz (Eds.), Behavior of nonhuman primates (Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press, 1965.
  • Hassell, M. P. The dynamics of competition and predation. London: Edward Arnold, 1976.
  • Hassell, M. P. The dynamics of arthropod predator-prey systems. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press, 1978.
  • Hatten, J. L., & Shull, R. L. Pausing on fixed-interval schedules: effects of the prior feeder duration. Behaviour Analysis Letters, 1983, 3, 101-111.
  • Hearst, E. The classical-instrumental distinction: reflexes, voluntary behavior, and cate­gories of associative learning. In W. K. Estes (Ed.), Handbook of learning and cognitive processes (Vol.2). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1975.
  • Hearst, E. Stimulus relationships and feature selection in learning and behavior. In S. H. Hulse, H. Fowler, & W. K. Honig (Eds.), Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1978.
  • Hearst, E. The first century of experimental psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1979.
  • Hearst, E., & Jenkins, H. M. Sign-tracking: the stimulus-reinforcer relation and directed action. Psychonomic Society Monograph, 1974.
  • Hearst, E., Koresko, M. B., & Poppen, R. Stimulus generalization and the response-reinforcement contingency. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1964, 7, 369-380.
  • Heinroth, O. Beiträge zur Biologie, namentlich Ethologie und Psychologie der Anatiden. Proceedings of the 5th International Ornithological Congress, 1911, 589-702.
  • Henderson, J. M., & Quandt, R. E. Microeconomic theory: a mathematical approach. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1971.
  • Henton, W. W., & Iversen, I. H. Classical conditioning and operant conditioning. New York: Springer-Verlag, 1978.
  • Richard J. Herrnstein:
    • Herrnstein, R. J. Relative and absolute strength of response as a function of frequency of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1961, 4, 267-272.
    • Herrnstein, R. J. Secondary reinforcement and rate of primary reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1964, 7, 27-36.
    • Herrnstein, R. J. On the law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1970, 13, 243-266.
    • Herrnstein, R. J. Self control as response strength. In C. M. Bradshaw, C. F. Lowe, & E. Szabadi (Eds.), Recent Developments in the quantification of steady-state operant behavior. Amsterdam: Elsevier/North-Holland, 1981.
    • Herrnstein, R. J. The matching law. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1997.
    • Herrnstein, R. J., & Boring, E. G. A source book in the history of psychology. Cam­bridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1965.
    • Herrnstein, R. J., & Heyman, G. M. Is matching compatible with reinforcement maximi­zation on concurrent variable interval, variable ratio? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1979, 31, 209-223.
    • Herrnstein, R. J., & Loveland, D. H. Complex visual concept in the pigeon. Science, 1964, 146, 549-551.
    • Herrnstein, R. J., & Loveland, D. H. Hunger and contrast in a multiple schedule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1974, 21, 511-517.
    • Herrnstein, R. J., & Loveland, D. H. Maximizing and matching on concurrent ratio schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1975, 24, 107-116.
    • Herrnstein, R. J., Loveland, D. H., & Cable, C. Natural concepts in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1976, 2, 285-302.
  • Hess, E. H. Space perception in the chick. Scientific American, 1956, 195, 71-80.
  • Hess, E. H. Imprinting. New York: Van Nostrand/Reinhold, 1973.
  • Hetherington, A. W., & Ranson, S. W. Hypothalamic lesions and adiposity in the rat. Anatomical Record, 1940, 78, 149-172.
  • Hetherington, A. W., & Ranson, S. W. The spontaneous activity and food intake of rats with hypothalamic lesions. American Journal of Physiology, 1942, 136, 609-617.
  • Heyman, G. M. Matching and maximizing in concurrent schedules. Psychological Re­view, 1979, 86, 496-500.
  • Hicks, J. R. A revision of demand theory. Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1956.
  • Hilgard, E. R., & Marquis, D. G. Conditioning and learning. New York: Appleton-­Century-Crofts, 1940.
  • Hinde, R. A. Bird vocalizations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1969.
  • Hinde, R. A. Animal behaviour: a synthesis of ethology and comparative psychology (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill, 1970.
  • Hinde, R. A., & Hinde, J. (Eds.) Constraints on learning: limitations and predisposi­tions. New York: Academic Press, 1973.
  • Hineline, P. N. Negative reinforcement and avoidance. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.
  • Hinson, J. M., & Staddon, J. E. R. Behavioral competition: a mechanism for schedule interactions. Science, 1978, 202, 432-434.
  • Hinson, J. M., & Staddon, J. E. R. Maximizing on interval schedules. In C. M. Bradshaw, C. F. Lowe, & E. Szabadi (Eds.), Recent developments in the quantification of steady-state operant behavior. Amsterdam: Elsevier/North-Holland, 1981.
  • Hinson, J. M., & Staddon, J. E. R. Hill-climbing by pigeons. Journal of the Experimen­tal Analysis of Behavior, 1983, 39, 25-47.
  • Hirsch, E., & Collier, G. The ecological determinants of reinforcement in the guinea pig. Physiology and Behavior, 1974, 12, 239-249.
  • Hodos, W., & Trumbule, G. H. Strategies of schedule preference in chimpanzees. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1967, 10, 503-514.
  • Hoebel, B. G., & Teitelbaum, P. Weight regulation in normal and hypothalamic hyper­phagic rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1966, 61, 189-193.
  • Hogan, J. A. How young chicks learn to recognize food. In R. A. Hinde & J. Stevenson­-Hinde (Eds.), Constraints on learning. New York: Academic Press, 1973.
  • Hogan, J. A., Kleist, S., & Hutchings, C. S. L. Display and food as reinforcers in the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens). Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1970, 70, 351-357.
  • Hogan, J. A., & Roper, T. J. A comparison of the properties of different reinforcers. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 1978, 8, 156-255.
  • Hollings, C. S. The components of predation, as revealed by a study of small mammal predation of the European pine sawfly. Canadian Entomologist, 1959, 91, 293-332.
  • Hollings, C. S. The functional response of predators to prey density and its role in mimicry and population regulation. Memoirs of the Entomological Society of Canada, 1965, 45, 5-60.
  • Holman, G. L. Intragastric reinforcement effect. Journal of Comparative and Physiologi­cal Psychology, 1969, 69, 432-441.
  • Honig, W. K. Studies of working memory in the pigeon. In S. H. Hulse, H. Fowler, & W. K. Honig (Eds.), Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1978.
  • Honig, W. K., Boneau, C. A., Burstein, K. R., & Pennypacker, H. S. Positive and negative generalization gradients obtained after equivalent training conditions. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1963, 56, 111-116.
  • Honig, W. K., & Urcuioli, P. J. The legacy of Guttman and Kalish (1956): 25 years of research on stimulus generalization. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1981, 36, 405-455.
  • Houston, A. I., & McNamara, J. How to maximize reward rate on two variable-interval paradigms. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1981, 35, 367-396.
  • Hovland, C. I. Human learning and retention. In S. S. Stevens (Ed.), Handbook of experimental psychology. New York: Wiley, 1951.
  • Hull, C. L. Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1943.
  • Hull, C. L. A behavior system. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1952.
  • Hull, C. L., Hovland, C. I., Ross, R. T., Hall, M., Perkins, D. T., & Fitch, F. B. Mathematico-deductive theory of rote learning. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1940.
  • Hulse, S. H., Deese, J., & Egeth, H. The psychology of learning. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1975.
  • Huxley, J. Problems of relative growth. London: Methuen, 1932. (Dover edition, 1972.).

I

  • Immelman, K. Sexual imprinting in birds. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 1972, 4,147-174.
  • Ingle, D. Focal attention in the frog: behavioral and physiological correlates. Science ,1975, 188, 1033-1035.
  • Intriligator, M. D. Mathematical optimization and economic theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1971.

J

  • Jenkins, H. M. Generalization gradients and the concept of inhibition. In D. I. Mostofsky (Ed.), Stimulus generalization. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press, 1965.
  • Jenkins, H. M., & Sainsbury, R. S. Discrimination learning with the distinctive feature on positive or negative trials. In D. I. Mostofsky (Ed.), Attention: contemporary theory and analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1970.
  • Jennings, H. S. Behavior of the lower organisms. Bloomington: Indiana University Press, 1976. (Reprint of the 1906 edition.)

K

  • Kacelnik, A., & Bateson, M. Risk-sensitivity: cross-roads for theories of decision-making. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1997, 1, 304-309.
  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. Prospect theory: an analysis of decision-making under risk. Econometrica, 1979, 47, 263-291.
  • Kamil, A. C. Systematic foraging by a nectar-feeding bird, the Amakihi (Loxops virens). Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1978, 92, 388-396.
  • Kamil, A. C., & Sargent, T. D. (Eds.) Foraging behavior: ecological, ethological, and psychological approaches. New York: Garland Press, 1981.
  • Kamin, L. J. Selective attention and conditioning. In N. J. Mackintosh & W. K. Honig (Eds.), Fundamental issues in associative learning. Halifax: Dalhousie University Press, 1969.
  • Keesey, R. E., & Powley, T. L. Hypothalamic regulation of body weight. American Scientist, 1975, 63, 558-565.
  • Keith-Lucas, T., & Guttman, N. Robust-single-trial delayed backward conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1975, 88, 468-476.
  • Kelleher, R. T. Chaining and conditioned reinforcement. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: areas of research and application. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1966.
  • Kello, J. E. The reinforcement-omission effect on fixed-interval schedules: frustration or inhibition? Learning Motivation, 1972, 3, 138-147.
  • Kello, J. Observation of the behavior of rats running to reward and nonreward in an alleyway. Doctoral dissertation, Duke University, 1973.
  • Kello, J. E., Innis, N. K., & Staddon, J. E. R. Eccentric stimuli on multiple fixed-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1975, 23, 233-240.
  • Kelsey, J. E., & Allison, J. Fixed ratio lever pressing by VMH rats: work vs. accessibility of sucrose reward. Physiology and Behavior, 1976, 17, 749-754.
  • Kennedy, G. C. The hypothalamic control of food intake in rats. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 1950, 137, 535-549.
  • Kennedy, J. S. Coordination of successive activities in an aphid: reciprocal effects of settling on flight. Journal of Experimental Biology, 1965, 43, 489-509.
  • Kennedy, J. S. Behaviour as physiology. In L. B. Browne (Ed.), Insects and physiology. New York: Springer-Verlag, 1967.
  • Killeen, P. R. On the temporal control of behavior. Psychological Review, 1975, 82, 89-115.
  • Killeen, P. R. Superstition: a matter of bias, not detectability. Science, 1978, 199, 88-90.
  • Killeen, P. R. Incentive theory. In D. Bernstein (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motiva­tion, 1981. Lincoln: Nebraska University Press, 1982.
  • Killeen, P. R. Incentive theory: II. models for choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1982, 38, 217-232.
  • Killeen, P. Complex dynamic processes in sign tracking with an omission contingency (negative automaintenance). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2003, 29, 49-61.
  • Killeen, P. R., & Fantino, E. Unification of models for choice between delayed reinforcers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1990, 53, 189-200.
  • Killeen, P. R., Hanson, S. J., & Osborne, S. R. Arousal: its genesis and manifestation as response rate. Psychological Review, 1978, 85, 571-581.
  • Kimble, G. A. Hilgard and Marquis' conditioning and learning (2nd ed). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1961.
  • Kimble, G. A., & Perlmutter, L. C. The problem of volition. Psychological Review, 1970, 77, 361-384.
  • Klopfer, P. H., & Gamble, J. Maternal "imprinting" in goats: the role of chemical senses. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 1966, 25, 588-592.
  • Koch, S. (Ed.) Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. 2). New York: McGraw-Hill, 1959.
  • Kolata, G. B. Obesity: a growing problem. Science, 1977, 198, 905-906.
  • Konishi, M., & Hauser, M. (Eds.) The design of animal communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1999.
  • Konishi, M., & Nottebohm, F. Experimental studies in the ontogeny of avian vocaliza­tions. In R. A. Hinde (Ed.), Bird vocalizations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1969.
  • Koshland, D. B. A response regulator model in a simple sensory system. Science, 1977, 196, 1055-1063.
  • Koshland, D. E. Bacterial chemotaxis as a model behavioral system. New York: Raven Press, 1980.
  • Kraly, F. S., & Blass, E. M. Increased feeding in rats in a low ambient temperature. In D. Novin, W. Wyrwicka, & G. A. Bray (Eds.), Hunger: basic mechanisms and clinical implications. New York: Raven Press, 1976.
  • Krebs, J. R. Behavioural aspects of predation. In P. P. G. Bateson & P. H. Klopfer (Eds.), Perspectives in ethology. New York: Plenum, 1973.
  • Krebs, J. R. Optimal foraging: decision rules for predators. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer, 1978.
  • Krebs, J. R., & Davies, N. B. (Eds.) Behavioural ecology. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer, 1978.
  • Krebs, J. R., & Davies, N. B. An introduction to behavioural ecology. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer, 1981.
  • Krebs, J. R., Kacelnik, A., & Taylor, P. Tests of optimal sampling by foraging great tits. Nature, 1978, 275, 27-31.
  • Kreider, D. L., Kuller, R. G., Ostberg, D. R., & Perkins, F. W. An introduction to linear analysis. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley, 1966.
  • Kroodsma, D. A reevaluation of song development in the song sparrow. Animal Behaviour, 1977, 25, 390-399.
  • Kroodsma, D. A. Aspects of learning in the ontogeny of bird song: where, from whom, when, how much, which, and how accurately? In G. Burghardt & M. Bekoff (Eds.), Development of behavior. New York: Garland Press, 1978.
  • Kuo, Z-Y. The dynamics of behavior development: an epigenetic view. New York: Random House, 1967.
  • Kuo, Z-Y. The need for coordinated efforts in developmental studies. In L. R. Aronson, E. Tobach, D. S. Lehrrnan, & J. S. Rosenblatt (Eds.), Development and evolution of behavior: essays in memory of T. C. Schneirla. San Francisco: Freeman, 1970.

L

  • Lancaster, K. Mathematical economics. New York: Macmillan, 1968.
  • Land, E. H., & McCann, J. J. Lightness and retinex theory. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 1971, 61, 1-11.
  • Landauer, T. K. Reinforcement as consolidation. Psychological Review, 1969, 76, 82-96.
  • Lashley, K. S. The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms and behavior. New York: Wiley, 1951.
  • Lea, S. E. G. Titration of schedule parameters by pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1976, 25, 43-54.
  • Lea, S. E. G. Concurrent fixed-ratio schedules for different reinforcers: a general theory. In C. M. Bradshaw, E. Szabadi, & C. F. Lowe (Eds.), Quantification of steady-state operant behavior. Amsterdam: Elsevier/North-Holland, 1981.
  • Lea, S. E. G., & Roper, T. J. Demand for food on fixed-ratio schedules as a function of the quality of concurrently available reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1977, 27, 371-380.
  • Lehrman, D. S. Semantic and conceptual issues in the nature-nurture problem. In L. R. Aronson, E. Tobach, D. S. Lehrman, & J. S. Rosenblatt (Eds.), Development and evolution of behavior: essays in memory of T. C. Schneirla. San Francisco: Freeman, 1970.
  • Lett, B. T. Long-delay learning in the T-maze. Learning and Motivation, 1975, 6, 80-90.
  • Lewin, R. Seeds of change in embryonic development. Science, 1981, 214, 42-44.
  • Lockhead, G. R. Identification and the form of multidimensional discrimination space. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1970, 85, 1-10.
  • Lockhead, G. R. Processing dimensional stimuli: a note. Psychological Review, 1972, 79, 410-419.
  • Locurto, C., Terrace, H. S., & Gibbon, J. (Eds.) Autoshaping and conditioning theory. New York: Academic Press, 1981.
  • Loeb, J. Forced movements, tropisms, and animal conduct. Philadelphia: Lippincott, 1918. (Dover edition, 1973.)
  • Lolordo, V. M., McMillan, J. C., & Riley, A. L. The effects upon food-reinforced pecking and treadle-pressing of auditory and visual signals for response-independent food. Learning and Motivation, 1974, 5, 24-41.
  • Lorenz, K. Companions as factors in the bird's environment. In R. Martin (trans.), Studies in animal and human behaviour, Vol. I. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1970.
  • Lowe, C. F., Davey, G. C., & Harzem, P. Effects of reinforcement magnitude on interval and ratio schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1974, 22, 553-560.
  • Lucas, C. A. The control of keypecks during automaintenance by pre-keypeck omission training. Animal Learning and Behavior, 1975, 3, 33-36.
  • Luce, R. D. Individual choice behavior: a theoretical analysis. New York: Wiley, 1959.
  • Luce, R. D. Detection and recognition. In R. D. Luce, R. R. Bush, & E. Galanter (Eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Wiley, 1963.
  • Luce, R. D. The choice axiom after twenty years. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 1977, 15, 215-233.
  • Luce, R. D., Bush, R. R., & Galanter, E. Handbook of mathematical psychology (3 vols.). New York: Wiley, 1963.
  • Luce, R. D., & Raiffa, H. Games and decisions. New York: Wiley, 1957.
  • Ludlow, A. R. The behaviour of a model animal. Behaviour, 1976, 108, 131-172.
  • Ludlow, A. R. The evolution and simulation of a decision maker. In F. M. Toates & T. R. Halliday (Eds.), The analysis of motivational processes. London: Academic Press, 1980.

M

  • MacColl, L. A. Fundamental theory of servomechanisms. New York: Van Nostrand, 1945.
  • MacEwen, D. The effects of terminal-link fixed-interval and variable-interval schedules on responding under concurrent chained schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1972, 18, 253-261.
  • Mackintosh, N. J. Comparative psychology of serial reversal and probability learning: rats, birds, and fish. In R. Gilbert & N. S. Sutherland (Eds.), Animal discrimination learning. London: Academic Press, 1969.
  • Mackintosh, N.J. The psychology of animal learning. New York: Academic Press, 1974. Mackintosh, N. J. A theory of animal attention: variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Review, 1975, 82, 276-298.
  • Mackintosh, N. J. Stimulus control: attentional factors. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.
  • Mackintosh, N. J., Bygrave, D. J., & Picton, B. M. B. Locus of the effects of a surprising reinforcer in the attenuation of blocking. Quarterly Journal of Psychol­ogy, 1977, 29, 327-336.
  • Macnab, R. M., & Koshland, D. E. The gradient-sensing mechanism in bacterial chemo­taxis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 1972, 69, 2509-2512.
  • Magnus, D. Experimentelle untersuchungen zur bionomie und ethologie des kaisermantels Argynnispaphia L. (Lep. Nymph.) Zeitschrift fürTierpsychologie, 1958, 15, 397-426.
  • Mahoney, W. J., & Ayres, J. J. B. One-trial simultaneous and backward fear condition­ing as reflected in conditioned suppression of licking in rats. Animal Learning and Behavior, 1976, 4, 357-362.
  • Maier, S. F., & Jackson, R. L. Learned helplessness: all of us were right (and wrong): learned helplessness has multiple effects. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 13). New York: Academic Press, 1979.
  • Malone, J. C., & Staddon, J. E. R. Contrast effects in maintained generalization gradi­ents. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1973, 19, 167-179.
  • Marler, P. R., & Hamilton, W. J. Mechanisms of animal behavior. New York: Wiley, 1966.
  • Marler, P., Kreith, M., & Tamura, M. Song development in hand-raised Oregon juncos. Auk, 1962, 79, 12-30.
  • Marler, P., & Tamura, M. Culturally transmitted patterns of vocal behavior in sparrows. Science, 1964, 46, 1483-1486.
  • Marshall, A. Principles of economics (8th ed.). London: Macmillan, 1925.
  • Marwine, A., & Collier, G. The rat at the waterhole. Journal of Comparative and Physio­logical Psychology, 1979, 93, 39l-402.
  • Mast, S. O. Light and the behavior of organisms. New York: Wiley, 1911.
  • Maxwell, J. C. On governors. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, 1868, 16, 270-283.
  • May, R. M. Simple mathematical models with very complicated dynamics. Nature, 1976, 261, 459-467.
  • Maynard Smith, J. Evolution and the theory of games. Scientific American, 1976, 64, 41-45.
  • Maynard Smith, J. Theory in evolution. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 1978, 9, 31-56.
  • McCleery, R. On satiation curves. Animal Behaviour, 1977, 25, 1005-1015.
  • McCleery, R. Optimal behaviour sequences and decision making. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural ecology. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer, 1978.
  • McFarland, D. J. Feedback mechanisms in animal behaviour. New York: Academic Press, 1971.
  • McFarland, D. J. (Ed.) Motivational control systems analysis. London: Academic Press, 1974.
  • McFarland, D. J., & Houston, A. Quantitative ethology: the state space approach. London: Pitman, 1981.
  • McGinty, D., Epstein, A. N., & Teitelbaum, P. The contribution of oropharyngeal sensations to hypothalamic hyperphagia. Animal Behaviour, 1965, 13, 413-418.
  • McKearney, J. W. Fixed-interval schedules of electric shock presentation: extinction and recovery of performance under different shock intensities and fixed-interval dura­tions. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1969, 12, 301-313.
  • McLean, A. P., & White, K. G. Temporal constraint on choice: Sensitivity and bias in multiple schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1983, 39, 405-426.
  • McNamara, J., & Houston, A. I. The application of statistical decision theory to animal behaviour. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 1980, 85, 673-690.
  • McNamera, J.M., & Houston, A. I. Models of adaptive behavior: an approach based on state. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1999.
  • McSweeney, F. K., & Melville, C. L. Behavioral contrast for key pecking as a function of component duration when only one component varies. Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 1993, 60, 331-343.
  • Medin, D. L. Form perception and pattern reproduction by monkeys. Journal of Compar­ative and Physiological Psychology, 1969, 68, 412-419.
  • Meschowski, H. Ways of thought of great mathematicians. San Francisco: Holden-Day, 1964.
  • Miles, R. C. Discrimination-learning sets. In A. M. Schrier, H. F. Harlow, & F. Stollnitz (Eds.), Behavior of nonhuman primates (Vol. I). New York: Academic Press, 1965.
  • Milich, R. S. A critical analysis of Schachter's externality theory of obesity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1975, 84, 586-588.
  • Miller, G.A. The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 1956, 63, 81-97.
  • Miller, N. E., & Banuazizi, A. Instrumental learning by curarized rats of a specific vis­ceral response. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1968, 65, 1-7.
  • Miller, N. E., & Dworkin, B. R. Visceral learning: recent difficulties with curarized rats and significant problems for human research. In P. A. Obrist, A. H. Black, J. Brener, & L. V. DiCara (Eds.), Cardiovascular physiology: current issues in re­sponse mechanisms, biofeedback, and methodology. Chicago: Aldine, 1974.
  • Milsum, J. H. Biological control systems analysis. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1966.
  • Minsky, M. Computation: finite and infinite machines. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1967.
  • MIT Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science. http://cognet.mit.edu/MITECS/Front/introduction.html.
  • Mittelstaedt, H. Basic solutions to a problem of angular orientation. In R. F. Reiss (Ed.), Neural theory and modeling. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press, 1964.
  • Mittelstaedt, H. Kybernetik der schwereorientierung. Verhandlungen der Deutschen Zoo­logischen Gesellschaft, 1972, 185-200.
  • Moore, E. F. Gedanken-experiments on sequential machines. In C. E. Shannon & J. McCarthy (Eds.), Automata studies. Princeton, NJ, Princeton Annals of Mathe­matics Studies, 1956.
  • Moore, G. E. Principia ethica. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1903.
  • Moorhouse, J. E., Fosbrooke, I. H. M., & Kennedy, J. S. "Paradoxical driving" of walking activity in locusts. Journal of Experimental Biology, 1978, 72, 1-16.
  • Morse, W., & Kelleher, R. Determinants of reinforcement and punishment. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.
  • Mrosovsky, N., & Powley, T. L. Set points for body weight and fat. Behavioral Biology, 1977, 20, 205-223.
  • Müller, A. Ãœber lichtreaktionen von landasseln. Zeitschrift für Vergleichende Physiologie, 1925, 3, 113-144.
  • Mulligan, J. A. Singing behavior and its development in the song sparrow, Melospiza melodia. University of California Publications in Zoology, 1966, 81, 1-76.
  • Murdoch, W. W. Switching in general predators: experiments on predator specificity and stability of prey populations. Ecological Monographs, 1969, 39, 335-354.
  • Murdoch, W. W., & Oaten, A. Predation and population stability. In A. Macfadyen (Ed.), Advances in ecological research (Vol. 9). New York: Academic Press, 1975.
  • Myerson, J., & Miezin, F. M. The kinetics of choice: an operant systems analysis. Psychological Review, 1980, 87, 160-174.

N

  • Navarick, D. J., & Fantino, E. Self-control and general models of choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1976, 2, 75-87.
  • Neimark, E. D., & Estes, W. K. Stimulus sampling theory. San Francisco: Holden-Day, 1967.
  • Neuringer, A. J., & Chung, S.-H. Quasi-reinforcement: control of responding by a percentage reinforcement schedule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behav­ior, 1967, 10, 45-54.
  • Nevin, J. A. Response strength in multiple schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analy­sis of Behavior, 1974, 21, 389-408.
  • Nevin, J. A. Overall matching versus momentary maximizing: Nevin (1969) revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1979a, 5, 300-306.
  • Nevin, J. A. Reinforcement schedules and response strength. In M. D. Zeiler, and P. Harzem (Eds.), Reinforcement and the organization of behavior. New York: Wiley, 1979b.
  • Nevin, J. A., & Grace, R. C. Behavioral momentum and the law of effect. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2000, 23, 73-130.
  • Nevin, J. A., & Shettleworth, S. J. An analysis of contrast effects in multiple schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1966, 9, 305-315.
  • Nevin, J. A., Tota, M. E., Torquato, R. D., & Shull, R. L. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1990, 53, 359-379.
  • Nicolaïdis, S., & Rowland, N. Intravenous self-feeding: long-term regulation of energy balance in rats. Science, 1977, 195, 589-591.
  • Nisbett, R. E. Hunger, obesity, and the ventromedial hypothalamus. Psychological Re­view, 1972, 79, 433-453.
  • Novin, D., Wyrwicka, W., & Bray, G. A. (Eds.) Hunger: basic mechanisms and clinical implications. New York: Raven Press, 1976.

O

  • Olton, D. S. Characteristics of spatial memory. In S. H. Hulse, H. Fowler, & W. K. Honig (Eds.), Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1978.
  • Olton, D. S., & Samuelson, R. J. Remembrance of places passed: spatial memory in rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1976, 2, 97-116.
  • Osgood, C. E. Method and theory in experimental psychology. New York: Oxford University Press, 1953.
  • Oster, J. F., & Wilson, E.O. Caste and ecology in the social insects. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1978.

P

  • Panksepp, J. Hypothalamic regulation of energy balance and feeding behavior. Federa­tion Proceedings, 1974, 33, 1150-1165.
  • Parker, G. A., & Stuart, R. A. Animal behavior as a strategy optimizer: evolution of resource assessment strategies and optimal emigration thresholds. American Naturalist, 1976, 110, 1055-1076.
  • Pavlov, I. P. Conditioned reflexes (G. V. Anrep, trans.). London: Oxford University Press, 1927.
  • Pearce, J. M., & Hall, G. A model for Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review, 1980, 87,532-552.
  • Peck, J. W. Situational determinants of the body weights defended by normal rats and rats with hypothalamic lesions. In D. Novin, W. Wyrwicka, & G. A. Bray (Eds.), Hunger: basic mechanisms and clinical implications. New York: Raven Press, 1976.
  • Peden, B. F., Browne, M. P., & Hearst, E. Persistent approaches to a signal for food despite food omission for approaching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1977, 3, 377-399.
  • Penney, J., & Schull, J. Functional differentiation of adjunctive drinking and wheel running in rats. Animal Learning and Behavior, 1977, 5, 272-280.
  • Pietrewicz, A. T., & Karnil, A. C. Search image formation in the blue jay (Cyanocitta cristata). Science, 1979, 204, 1332-1333.
  • Pinker, S. The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature. New York: Viking, 2002.
  • Pittendrigh, C. S. Adaptation, natural selection, and behavior. In A. Roe & G. G. Simpson (Eds.), Behavior and evolution. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1958.
  • Popper, K. The philosophy of Karl Popper. In P. Schilpp (Ed.), The library of living philosophers (Vol. 14). LaSalle, IL: Open Court, 1974.
  • Popper, K. R., & Eccles, J. C. The self and its brain. Berlin, W. Germany: Springer, 1977.
  • Posner, M. I. Chronometric explorations of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1978.
  • Posner, M. I., & Shulman, G. L. Cognitive science. In E. Hearst (Ed.), The first century of experimental psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1979.
  • Postman, L. The history and present status of the law of effect. Psychological Bulletin, 1947, 44, 489-563.
  • Poulton, E. B. Proof of the protective value of dimorphism in larvae. Transactions of the Entomological Society, London, 1888, 9, 50-56.
  • Poulton, E. C. The new psychophysics: six models for magnitude estimation. Psycholog­ical Bulletin, 1968, 69, 1-19.
  • Powers, W. T. Quantitative analysis of purposive systems: some spadework on the foundations of scientific psychology. Psychological Review, 1978, 85, 4l7-435.
  • Powley, T. L., & Opsahl, C. A. Autonomic components of the hypothalamic feeding syndromes. In D. Novin, W. Wyrwicka, & G. A. Bray (Eds.), Hunger: basic mechanisms and clinical implications. New York: Raven Press, 1976.
  • Premack, D. Reinforcement theory. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motiva­tion (Vol. 13). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1965.
  • Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. Increased eating in rats deprived of running. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1963, 6, 209-212.
  • Pulliam, H. R. Diet optimization with nutrient constraints. American Naturalist, 1975, 109, 765-768.
  • Pyke, G. H., Pulliam, H. R., & Charnov, E. L. Optimal foraging: a selective review of theory and tests. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 1977, 52, 137-154.

R

  • Rabbitt, P. Sorting, categorization, and visual search. In E. C. Carterette & M. P. Friedman (Eds.), Handbook of perception, Vol. XI. New York: Academic Press, 1978.
  • Rachlin, H. Introduction to modern behaviorism. San Francisco: Freeman, 1970.
  • Rachlin, H. Contrast and matching. Psychological Review, 1973, 80, 217-234.
  • Rachlin, H. A molar theory of reinforcement schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1978, 30, 345-360.
  • Rachlin, H., & Burkhard, B. The temporal triangle: response substitution in instrumental conditioning. Psychological Review, 1978, 85, 22-48.
  • Rachlin, H., & Green, L. Commitment, choice and self-control. Journal of the Experi­mental Analysis of Behavior, 1972, 17, 15-22.
  • Rachlin, H., Green, L., Kagel, J. H., & Battalio, R. C. Economic demand theory and psychological studies of choice. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 10). New York: Academic Press, 1976.
  • Ranson, S. W., & Clark, S. L. The anatomy of the nervous system. Philadelphia: Saunders, 1953. (Reprinted, 1958.)
  • Ratliff, F. Mach bands: quantitative studies on neural networks in the retina. San Francisco: Holden-Day, 1965.
  • Ratliff, F. Studies in excitation and inhibition in the retina: a collection of papers from the laboratories of H. K. Hartline. New York: Rockefeller University Press, 1974.
  • Real, L. A. The kinetics of functional response. American Naturalist, 1977, 111, 289-300.
  • Real, L. A. On uncertainty and the law of diminishing returns in evolution and behavior. In J. E. R. Staddon (Ed.), Limits to action: the allocation of individual behavior. New York: Academic Press, 1980.
  • Alliston K. Reid:
    • Reid, A. K., & Dale, R. H. I. Dynamic effects of food magnitude on interim-terminal interactions. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1983, 39, 135-148.
    • Reid, A. K., & Staddon, J. E. R. Schedule-induced drinking: elicitation, anticipation, or behavioral interaction? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1982, 38, 1-18.
  • Rescorla, R. A. Pavlovian conditioning and its proper control procedures. Psychological Review, 1967, 74, 71-80.
  • Rescorla, R. A. Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear condition­ing. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1968, 66, 1-5.
  • Rescorla, R. A. Pavlovian excitatory and inhibitory conditioning. In W. K. Estes (Ed.), Handbook of learning and cognitive processes (Vol. 2). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1975.
  • Rescorla, R. A. Stimulus generalization: some predictions from a model of Pavlovian conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1976, 2, 88-96.
  • Rescorla, R. A. Behavioral studies of Pavlovian conditioning. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 1988, 11, 329-352.
  • Rescorla, R. A., & Skucy, J. C. Effect of response-independent reinforcers during extinction. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1969, 67, 381-389.
  • Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A. Black & W. R. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1972.
  • Revusky, S. H. The role of interference in association over delay. In W. K. Honig & P. H. R. James (Eds.), Animal memory. New York: Academic Press, 1971.
  • Revusky, S. H. Learning as a general process with emphasis on data from feeding experiments. In N. W. Milgram, L. Krames, & T. M. Alloway (Eds.), Food aver­sion learning. New York: Plenum, 1977.
  • Reynolds, G. S. Attention in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behav­ior, 1961, 4, 203-208. (a)
  • Reynolds, G. S. Behavioral contrast. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1961, 4, 57-71. (b)
  • Reynolds, G. S. On some determinants of choice in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1963, 6, 53-59. (a)
  • Reynolds, G. S. Some limitations on behavioral contrast and induction during successive discrimination. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1963, 6, 131-139. (b)
  • Richards, J. J., & Blackman, D. E. Effect of time-out duration after food reinforcement in a fixed-ratio schedule with pigeons. Behaviour Analysis Letters, 1981, 1, 81-87.
  • Richardson, M. W. Multidimensional psychophysics. Psychological Bulletin, 1938, 35, 659-660.
  • Richelle, M. B. F. Skinner, a reappraisal. Hove, UK: Erlbaum, 1995.
  • Richelle, M., & Lejeune, H. Time in animal behaviour. Oxford: Pergamon, 1980.
  • Riley, D. A., & Roitblat, H. L. Selective attention and related processes in pigeons. In S. H. Hulse, H. Fowler, & W. K. Honig (Eds.), Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1978.
  • Rilling, M. Stimulus control and inhibitory processes. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.
  • Roberts, W. A. Spatial separation and visual differentiation of cues as factors influencing short-term memory in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychol­ogy, 1972, 78, 281-291.
  • Roberts, W. A., & Dale, R. H. I. Remembrance of places lasts: proactive inhibition and patterns of choice in rat spatial memory. Learning and Motivation, 1981, 12, 261-281.
  • Roberts, W. A., & Grant, D. S. Short-term memory in the pigeon with presentation time precisely controlled. Learning and Motivation, 1974, 5, 393-408.
  • Rodgers, W., & Rozin, P. Novel food preferences in thiamine-deficient rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1966, 61, l-4.
  • Roitblat, H. L. The meaning of representation in animal memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1982, 5, 353-406.
  • Roper, T. J. What is meant by the term “schedule-induced," and how general is schedule induction? Animal Learning and Behavior, 1981, 9, 433-440.
  • Rothkopf, E. Z. A measure of stimulus similarity and errors in some paired-associate learning tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1957, 53, 94-l0l.
  • Rosenblueth, A., Wiener, N., & Bigelow, J. Behavior, purpose and teleology. Philoso­phy of Science, 1943, 10, 18-24.
  • Rozin, P., & Kalat, J. W. Specific hungers and poison avoidance as adaptive specializa­tions of learning. Psychological Review, 1971, 78, 459-486.
  • Russek, M. A conceptual equation of intake control. In D. Novin, W. Wyrwicka, & G. A. Bray (Eds.), Hunger: basic mechanisms and clinical implications. New York: Raven Press, 1976.
  • Bertrand Russell:
    • Russell, B. History of western philosophy. London: Allen & Unwin, 1946.
    • Russell, B. The basic writings of Bertrand Russell. In R. E. Egner & L. E. Denonn (Eds.), New York: Simon & Schuster, 1961.

S

  • Sainsbury, R. S. Effect of proximity of elements on the feature-positive effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1971, 16, 315-325.
  • Samuelson, P. A. Foundations of economic analysis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni­versity Press, 1965.
  • Schachter, S., & Rodin, J. Obese humans and rats. Potomac, MD: Erlbaum, 1974.
  • Schick, K. Operants. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1971, 15, 413-423.
  • Schneider, B. A. A two-state analysis of fixed-interval responding in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1969, 12, 677-687.
  • Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. Controlled and automatic human information pro­cessing: I. detection, search and attention. Psychological Review, 1977, 84, 1-66.
  • Schoener, T. W. Theory of feeding strategies. Annual Review of Ecology and Systemat­ics, 1971, 2, 369-404.
  • Schutz, F. Prägung des sexual verhaltens von enten und gansen durch sozialeindrticke wirhend derjugendphase. Journal of Neurovisceral Relations, Supplementum, 1971, 10, 399-457.
  • Schwartz, B., & Gamzu, E. Pavlovian control of operant behavior. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.
  • Sclafani, A., & Springer, D. Dietary obesity in adult rats: similarities to hypothalamic and human obesity syndromes. Physiology and Behavior, 1976, 17, 461-471.
  • Sclafani, A., Springer, D., & Kluge, L. Effects of quinine adulterated diets on the food intake and body weight of obese and non-obese hypothalamic hyperphagic rats. Physiology and Behavior, 1976, 16, 631-640.
  • Seligman, M. E. P., & Hager, J. L. (Eds.) Biological boundaries of learning. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1972.
  • Seligman, M. E. P., & Maier, S. F. Failure to escape traumatic shock. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1967, 74, 1-9.
  • Seward, J. P. An experimental analysis of latent learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1949, 39,177-186.
  • Shannon, C., & Weaver, W. The mathematical theory of communication. Urbana: Univer­sity of Illinois Press, 1949.
  • Shaw, G. B. The black girl in search of God: and some lesser tales. Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin, 1946. (Reprinted from The adventures of the black girl in her search for God, first published in 1932.)
  • Shepard, R. N. Analysis of proximities as a technique for the study of information processing in man. Human Factors, 1963, 5, 33-48.
  • Shepard, R. N. Attention and the metric structure of the stimulus space. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 1964, 1, 54-87.
  • Shepard, R. N. Approximation to uniform gradients of generalization by monotone transformations of scale. In D. I. Mostofsky (Ed.), Stimulus generalization. Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1965.
  • Shepard, R. N. Farm, formation, and transformation of internal representations. In R. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: the Loyola symposium. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1975.
  • Shepard, R. N. Multidimensional scaling, tree-fitting, and clustering. Science, 1980, 210, 390-398.
  • Shepard, R. N., & Cooper, L. A. Mental images and their rotations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1982.
  • Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, B. Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 1971, 171, 701-703.
  • Sherrington, C. S. The integrative action of the nervous system. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1906. (Reprinted, 1947.)
  • Shettleworth, S. J. Constraints on learning. In D. S. Lehrman, R. A. Hinde, & E. Shaw (Eds.), Advances in the study of behavior (Vol. IV). New York: Academic Press, 1972.
  • Shettleworth, S. J. Reinforcement and the organization of behavior in golden hamsters: hunger, environment and food reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Psychology,1975, 104, 56-87.
  • Shettleworth, S. J. Reinforcement and the organization of behavior in golden hamsters: punishment of three action patterns. Learning and Motivation, 1978, 9, 99-123.
  • Shettleworth, S. J.Cognition, evolution and behavior. New York: Oxford, 1998.
  • Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. Controlled and automatic human information process­ing: II. perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychological Review, 1977, 84, 127-190.
  • Shimp, C. P. Probabilistically reinforced choice behavior in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1966, 9, 443-455.
  • Shimp, C. P. Optimal behavior in free-operant experiments. Psychological Review, 1969, 76, 97-112.
  • Shimp, C. P., & Wheatley, K. L. Matching to relative reinforcement frequency in multiple schedules with a short component duration. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1971, 15, 205-210.
  • Sibly, R., & McFarland, D. On the fitness of behavior sequences. American Naturalist, 1976, 110, 601-617.
  • Sidman, M. Tactics of scientific research: evaluating experimental data in psychology. New York: Basic Books, 1960.
  • Silberberg, A., Hamilton, B., Ziriax, J. M., & Casey, J. The structure of choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1978, 4, 368-398.
  • Simon, H. A. A note on Jost's law and exponential forgetting. Psychometrika, 1966, 31, 505-506.
  • Simon, H. A. Models of thought. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1979.
  • B. F. Skinner:
    • Skinner, B. F. On the rate of formation of a conditioned reflex. Journal of General Psychology, 1932, 7, 274-286.
    • Skinner, B. F. The generic nature of the concepts of stimulus and response. Journal of General Psychology, 1935, 12, 40-65.
    • Skinner, B. F. The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1938.
    • Skinner, B. F. "Superstition" in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1948, 38, 168-172.
    • Skinner, B. F. Are theories of learning necessary? Psychological Review, 1950, 57, 193-216.
    • Skinner, B. F. The phylogeny and ontogeny of behavior. Science, 1966, 153, 1205-1213.
  • Snowdon, C. T. Motivation, regulation, and the control of meal parameters with oral and intragastric feeding. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1969, 69, 91-100.
  • Spalding, D. A. Instinct: with original observations on young animals. Macmillan's Magazine, 1873, 27, 282-293.
  • Spence, K. The differential response in animals to stimuli varying in a single dimension. Psychological Review, 1937, 44, 435-444.
  • Sperry, R. W. Mechanisms of neural maturation. In S. S. Stevens (Ed.), Handbook of experimental psychology. New York: Wiley, 1951.
  • Sperry, R. W. A modified concept of consciousness. Psychological Review, 1969, 76, 532-536.
  • Squires, N., & Fantino, E. A model for choice in simple concurrent and concurrent-chains schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1971, 15, 27-38.
  • Squires, N., Norborg, J., & Fantino, E. Second-order schedules: discrimination of com­ponents. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1975, 24, 157-171.
  • J.E.R. Staddon:
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Some properties of spaced responding in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1965, 8, 19-27.
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Asymptotic behavior: the concept of the operant. Psychological Review, 1967, 74, 377-391.
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Multiple fixed-interval schedules: transient contrast and temporal inhibition. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1969, 12, 583-590.
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Temporal effects of reinforcement: a negative "frustration" effect. Learning and Motivation, 1970, 1, 227-247. (a)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Effect of reinforcement duration on fixed-interval responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1970, 13, 9-11. (b)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Reinforcement omission on temporal go-no-go schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1972, 18, 223-229. (a)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Temporal control and the theory of reinforcement schedules. In R. M. Gilbert & J. R. Millenson (Eds.), Reinforcement: behavioral analyses. New York: Academic Press, 1972. (b)
    • Staddon, J. E R. On the notion of cause, with applications to behaviorism. Behaviorism, 1973, 1, 25-63.
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Temporal control, attention and memory. Psychological Review, 1974, 81, 375-391.
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Limitations on temporal control: generalization and the effects of context. British Journal of Psychology, 1975, 66, 229-246. (a)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. A note on the evolutionary significance of supernormal stimuli. Ameri­can Naturalist, 1975, 109, 541-545. (b)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Schedule-induced behavior. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977. (a)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Behavioral competition in conditioning situations: notes toward a theory of generalization and inhibition. In H. Davis & H. M. B. Hurwitz (Eds.), Operant-Pavlovian interactions. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1977. (b)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Theory of behavioral power functions. Psychological Review, 1978, 85, 305-320.
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Operant behavior as adaptation to constraint. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1979, 108, 48-67. (a)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Regulation and time allocation: a commentary on “conservation in behavior." Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1979, 108, 35-40. (b)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. (Ed.) Limits to action: the allocation of individual behavior. New York: Academic Press, 1980. (a)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Optimality analyses of operant behavior and their relation to optimal foraging. In J. E. R. Staddon (Ed.), Limits to action: the allocation of individual behavior. New York: Academic Press, 1980. (b)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. On a possible relation between cultural transmission and genetical evolution. In P. Klopfer & P. P. G. Bateson (Eds.), Perspectives in ethology. Vol.4: Advantages of diversity. London: Plenum, 1981. (a)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Cognition in animals: learning as program assembly. Cognition, 1981, 10, 287-294. (b)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Behavioral competition, contrast, and matching. In M. L. Commons, R. J. Herrnstein, & H. Rachlin (Eds.), Quantitative analyses of operant behavior: matching and maximizing accounts. Quantitative analyses of behavior, Vol.II (five-volume series). Cambridge, MA: Ballinger, 1982.
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Inference, memory and representation. In N.M. Weinberger, J.L. McGaugh, & G. Lynch (Eds.) Memory systems of the brain: animal and human cognitive processes. New York: Guilford Publications, 1985.
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Quasi-dynamic choice models: melioration and ratio-invariance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1988, 49, 303-320.
    • Staddon, J.E.R. The new behaviorism: mind, mechanism and society. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press, 2001. (a)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. Adaptive dynamics: the theoretical analysis of behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT/Bradford, 2001. (b)
    • Staddon, J. E. R. (in press) Humanism and Skinner’s Radical Behaviorism. In Behavior Theory and Philosophy, Ed. Lattal & Chase.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Ayres, S. Sequential and temporal properties of behavior in­duced by a schedule of periodic food delivery. Behaviour, 1975, 54, 26-49.
    • Staddon, J. E. R. & Cerutti, D. T. Operant behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 2003, 54, 115-144.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Frank, J. Mechanisms of discrimination reversal. Animal Behaviour, 1974, 22, 802-828.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Gendron, R. P. Optimal detection of cryptic prey may lead to predator switching. American Naturalist, 1983, 122, 843-848.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Higa, J. J. Multiple time scales in simple habituation. Psychological Review, 1996, 103, 720-733.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Higa, J. J. Time and memory: towards a pacemaker-free theory of interval timing. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1999, 71, 215-251.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., Hinson, J. M., & Kram, R. Optimal choice. Journal of the Experimen­tal Analysis of Behavior, 1981, 35, 397-412.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Horner, J. M. Stochastic choice models: a comparison between Bush-Mosteller and a source-independent reward-following model. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1989, 52, 57-64.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Innis, N. K. Reinforcement omission on fixed-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1969, 12, 689-700.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Motheral, S. On matching and maximizing in operant choice experiments. Psychological Review, 1978, 85, 436-444.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Motheral, S. Response independence, matching, and maximizing: a reply to Heyman. Psychological Review, 1979, 86, 501-505.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Simmelhag, V. L. The "superstition" experiment: a reexamination of its implications for the principles of adaptive behavior. Psychological Review,1971, 78, 3-43.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Zanutto, B. S. Feeding dynamics: why rats eat in meals and what this means for foraging and feeding regulation. In M.E. Bouton & M.S. Fanselow (Eds.), Learning, motivation and cognition: the functional behaviorism of Robert C. Bolles. Washington: American Psychological Association, 1997.
    • Staddon, J. E. R., & Zhang, Y. On the assignment-of-credit problem in operant learning. In M. L. Commons, S. Grossberg, & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Neural networks of conditioning and action, the XIIth Harvard Symposium. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates, 1991.
  • Starr, B., & Staddon, J. E. R. Temporal control on fixed-interval schedules: signal-properties of reinforcement and blackout. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1974, 22, 535-545.
  • Stevens, S. S. On the averaging of data. Science, 1955, 121, 113-116.
  • Stigler, G. J. The development of utility theory. II. The Journal of Political Economy, 1950, 58, 373-396.
  • Stubbs, D. A. Second-order schedules and the problem of conditioned reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1971, 16, 289-313.
  • Stubbs, D. A. Response bias and the discrimination of stimulus duration. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1976, 25, 243-250.
  • Sutherland, N. S., & Mackintosh, N. J. Mechanisms of animal discrimination learning. New York: Academic Press, 1971.
  • Suzuki, S., Augerinos, G., & Black, A. H. Stimulus control of spatial behavior on the eight-arm maze in rats. Learning and Motivation, 1980, 6, 77-81.
  • Swazey, J. P. Reflexes and motor integration: Sherrington's concept of integrative ac­tion. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1969.
  • Symposia of the Society for Experimental Biology, No. XVIII: Homeostasis and feedback mechanisms. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 1964.

T

  • Teghtsoonian, R. On the exponents in Stevens' law and the constants in Ekman's law. Psychological Review, 1971, 78, 71-80.
  • Teitelbaum, P., & Epstein, A. N. The lateral hypothalamic syndrome. Psychological Review, 1962, 69, 74-90.
  • Terrace, H. S. Stimulus control. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: areas of research and application. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1966.
  • Thielcke, G. A. Bird sounds. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press, 1976.
  • Thompson, D. W. On growth and form. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1961. (Abridgment of the 1917 and 1942 editions, edited by J. T. Bonner.)
  • Thompson, R. F., & Spencer, W. A. Habituation: a model phenomenon for the study of neuronal substrates of behavior. Psychological Review, 1966, 73, 1643.
  • Thorndike, E. L. Animal intelligence: an experimental study of the associative processes in animals. Psychological Monographs, 1898, 2, 109.
  • Thorndike, E. L. Animal intelligence. New York: Macmillan, 1911.
  • Timberlake, W. A molar equilibrium theory of learned performance. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 14). New York: Academic Press, 1980.
  • Timberlake, W. Biological behaviorism. In W. O’Donohue & R. Kitchener (Eds.), Handbook of Behaviorism. San Diego: Academic Press, 1999.
  • Timberlake, W., & Allison, J. Response deprivation: an empirical approach to instru­mental performance. Psychological Review, 1974, 8, 146-164.
  • Tinbergen, L. The natural control of insects in pinewoods: I. factors affecting the inten­sity of predation by songbirds. Archives Neerlandaises de Zoologie, 1960, 13, 265-343.
  • Tinbergen, N. Social releasers and the experimental method required for their study. Wilson Bulletin, 1948, 60, 6-51.
  • Tinbergen, N. The study of instinct. New York: Oxford University Press, 1951.
  • Tinbergen, N. Curious naturalists. New York: Basic Books, 1958.
  • Tinbergen, N. On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 1963, 20, 410-433.
  • Tinbergen, N., Impekoven, M., & Franck, D. An experiment on spacing out as a defense against predators. Behaviour, 1967, 28, 307-321.
  • Toates, F. Control theory in biology and experimental psychology. London: Hutchinson Educational, 1975.
  • Toates, F. M., & Archer, J. A comparative review of motivational systems using classical control theory. Animal Behaviour, 1978, 26, 368-380.
  • Toates, F. M., & Halliday, T. R. (Eds.) The analysis of motivational processes. London: Academic Press, 1980.
  • Todorov, J. C. Component duration and relative response rates in multiple schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1972, 17, 45-49.
  • Todorov, J. C. Interaction of frequency and magnitude of reinforcement on concurrent performances. Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 1973, 19, 451-458.
  • Tolman, E. C. Purposive behavior in animals and men. New York: Appleton-Century­-Crofts, 1932. (Reprinted, University of California Press, 1949.)
  • Tolman, E. C., & Honzik, C. H. Introduction and removal of reward, and maze perfor­mance in rats. University of California Publications in Psychology, 1930, 4, 257-275.
  • Torgerson, W. S. Multidimensional scaling: theory and method. Psychometrika, 1952, 17, 401-419.
  • Toulmin, S. Neuroscience and human understanding. In G. Quarton, F. Schmitt, & E. Melnechuk (Eds.), Neuroscience: a study program. New York: Rockefeller Univer­sity Press, 1967

U-V

  • Uexküll, J. von. Streifzuge durch die Umwelten von Tieren undMenschen. Berlin: Springer,1934.
  • Uhl, C. N. Response elimination in rats with schedules of omission training, including yoked and response-independent reinforcement comparisons. Learning and Motivation, 1974, 5, 511-531
  • Verplanck, W. S. Since learned behavior is innate, and vice versa, what now? Psychological Review, 1955, 52, 139-144.
  • Von Neuman, J., & Morgenstern, O. Theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1947.

W

  • Waddington, C. H. Genetic assimilation of the Bithorax phenotype. Evolution, 1956, 10 ,1-13.
  • Waddington, C. H. Evolutionary adaptation. In S. Tax (Ed.), Evolution after Darwin. Vol.1: The evolution of life. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1960.
  • Waddington, C. H. New patterns in genetics and development. London: Allen & Unwin, 1962.
  • Wagner, A. R. Expectancies and the priming of STM. In S. H. Hulse, H. Fowler, & W. K. Honig (Eds.), Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1978.
  • Wagner, A. R., Rudy, J. W., & Whitlow, J. W. Rehearsal in animal conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1973, 97, 407-426.
  • Wagner, A. R., & Terry, W. S. Backward conditioning to a CS following an expected vs. a surprising UCS. Animal Learning and Behavior, 1975, 3, 370-374.
  • Wallsten, T. S. (Ed.) Cognitive processes in choice and decision behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1980.
  • Walsh, V. C. Introduction to contemporary microeconomics. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1970.
  • Watson, J. B. Psychology from the standpoint of a behaviorist. Philadelphia: Lippincott, 1919.
  • Werner, E. E., & Hall, D. J. Optimal foraging and the size selection of prey by the Bluegill Sunfish (Lepomis macrochirus). Ecology, 1974, 55, 1216-1232.
  • Wessells, M. G. The effects of reinforcement upon the pre-pecking behaviors of pigeons in the autoshaping experiment. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1974, 21, 125-144.
  • West, M. J., King, A. P., Eastzer, D. H., & Staddon, J. E. R. A bioassay of isolate cowbird song. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1979, 93, l24-l33.
  • Whipple, W. R., & Fantino, E. Key-peck durations under behavioral contrast and differ­ential reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1980, 34, 167-176.
  • White, K. G. Behavioral contrast as differential time allocation. Journal of the Experi­mental Analysis of Behavior, 1978, 29, 151-160.
  • Whitham, T. G. Coevolution of foraging in Bombus and nectar dispensing in Cilopsis: a last dreg theory. Science, 1977, 197, 593-596.
  • Wickens, D. D., & Wickens, C. D. Some factors related to pseudoconditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1942, 31, 518-526.
  • Wilcoxon, H. C. Historical introduction to the problem of reinforcement. In J. C. Tapp (Ed.), Reinforcement and behavior. New York: Academic Press, 1969.
  • Williams, B. A. Reinforcement, choice, and response strength. In R. C. Atkinson, R. J. Herrnstein, G. Lindzey, & R. D. Luce (Eds.), Stevens' handbook of experimental psychology (2 ed.,). New York: John Wiley, 1988.
  • Williams, D. R. Classical conditioning and incentive motivation. In W. F. Prokasy (Ed.), Classical conditioning: a symposium. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1965.
  • Williams, D. R., & Williams, H. Auto-maintenance in the pigeon: sustained pecking despite contingent non-reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Be­havior, 1969, 12, 511-520.
  • Wilson, M. P. Periodic reinforcement interval and number of periodic reinforcements as parameters of response strength. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1954, 47, 51-56.
  • Wirtshafter, D., & Davis, J. D. Set points, settling points, and the control of body weight. Physiology and Behavior, 1977, 19, 75-78.
  • Wynne, C. D. L., & Staddon, J. E. R. Typical delay determines waiting time on periodic-food schedules: static and dynamic tests. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1988, 50, 197-210.

Y-Z

  • Yates, F. A. The art of memory. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1966.
  • Zeiler, M. D., & Buchman, I. B. Response requirements as constraints on output. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1979, 32, 29-49.
  • Zeiler, M. D., & Harzem, P. (Eds.) Reinforcement and the organization of behavior. New York: Wiley, 1979.
  • Zener, K. The significance of behavior accompanying conditioned salivary secretion for theories of the conditioned response. American Journal of Psychology, 1937, 50, 384-403.
  • Zippelius, H. Die karawanenbildung bei feld- und hausspitzmaus. Zeitschrifi für Tierpsychologie, 1972, 30, 305-320.
  • Ziriax, J. M., & Silberberg, A. Discrimination and emission of different key-peck dura­tions in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Pro­cesses, 1978, 4, 1-21.